全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1224篇 |
免费 | 211篇 |
国内免费 | 52篇 |
专业分类
1487篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 22篇 |
2023年 | 19篇 |
2022年 | 32篇 |
2021年 | 43篇 |
2020年 | 46篇 |
2019年 | 51篇 |
2018年 | 67篇 |
2017年 | 52篇 |
2016年 | 48篇 |
2015年 | 38篇 |
2014年 | 39篇 |
2013年 | 98篇 |
2012年 | 76篇 |
2011年 | 94篇 |
2010年 | 71篇 |
2009年 | 74篇 |
2008年 | 83篇 |
2007年 | 101篇 |
2006年 | 93篇 |
2005年 | 75篇 |
2004年 | 44篇 |
2003年 | 31篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 8篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有1487条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
本文结合有关宗教学理论,对宋元净明道所推崇的"净明"、"忠孝"概念进行了解析.作者以为:宋元净明道视世俗性的道德实践为"神圣"的宗教修炼,实与宗教学中的凡俗可以"转换"为神圣之理论相合;而净明道徒的这种"转换",则多与其个体的"宗教体验"有关.至于元代净明道借"无极"概念来释"净明",以"净明"为回复宇宙根源之境界,则合于宗教人经验到与万物的根源、终极的本体或神圣的世界契合之感.换言之,宋元净明道的"净明"、"忠孝"说,实与各种宗教的本质有着相通之处. 相似文献
92.
Yanbo Wang Hongbiao Wang Ji Wang Jing Wu Xiaohong Liu 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2013,20(1):3-12
This study examined prevalence and predictors of posttraumatic growth in 180 accidentally injured patients of mainland China in their convalescence stage, investigating its relationships with demographic and accidental injury variables, personality, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms, and coping styles. Our results showed that posttraumatic growth (PTG) presented mostly in the domain of Relating to Others and indicated that PTG was significantly related to marital status, educational level, personality, coping styles, and PTSD symptoms. Avoidance of PTSD symptoms, Openness to experience, and positive coping were significant predictors of PTG. The findings emphasize that when promoting PTG of accidentally injured patients, healthcare providers should facilitate patients utilizing personal resources, understand PTG coexists with PTSD symptoms, and adjust interventions based on the coping styles the patients have adopted. 相似文献
93.
Background and objectives: Few studies related to the impact of different emotion regulation strategies on anxiety have used externally and ecologically valid emotion-eliciting stimuli or Eastern populations. The present study compares the effects of reappraisal, suppression, and acceptance on anxiety induced by a simulated job interview in a Chinese sample. Methods: Eighty-two subjects were randomly assigned to one of four instructions: reappraisal, suppression, acceptance, or no-regulation strategies during a simulated job interview. Anxiety was assessed with an observer-based behavior rating scale during the interview and the State Anxiety Inventory before, during, and after the interview. Results: A repeated-measures MANOVA indicated a significantly greater reduction in anxiety in the reappraisal and acceptance groups compared to the control group during the interview (reappraisal: d = 1.42; acceptance: d = 1.30; each p < .001), but not during the recovery stage. The suppression and control group did not differ in any stage. Suppression led to a higher (pmax < .04) anxiety than reappraisal/acceptance in the anticipation (d = 0.65/0.68), interview (d = 0.87/0.79), and recovery stages (d = 0.94/1.03). No significant differences were found between reappraisal and acceptance. Conclusions: In Chinese students reappraisal and acceptance seem to be more effective anxiety regulation strategies than suppression. 相似文献
94.
95.
96.
The Stroop effect has been shown to depend on the relative proportion of congruent and incongruent trials. This effect is
commonly attributed to experiment-wide word-reading strategies that change as a function of proportion congruent. Recently,
Jacoby, Lindsay, and Hessels (2003) reported an itemspecific proportion congruent effect that cannot be due to these strategies
and instead may reflect rapid, stimulus driven control over word-reading processes. However, an item-specific proportion congruent
effect may also reflect learned associations between color word identities and responses. In two experiments, we demonstrate
a context-specific proportion congruent effect that cannot be explained by such word—response associations. Our results suggest
that processes other than learning of word—response associations can produce contextual control over Stroop interference. 相似文献
97.
思维风格测验在大学生中的初步应用 总被引:20,自引:1,他引:20
本文介绍了斯腾伯格的心理自我管理理论并使用斯腾伯格所编制的思维风格问卷对大学生的思维风格进行了测量,比较了思维风格在性别、文理科、地域等方面的个体差异,得出了中国大学生在思维风格方面的一些特点;研究了思维风格与一般智力的关系,发现思维风格与智力相关很低,说明思维风格是一种独立于智力的特质;同时对思维风格量表的信、效度及量表的结构进行了探讨。 相似文献
98.
People consider the mental states of other people to understand their actions. We evaluated whether such perspective taking is culture dependent. People in collectivistic cultures (e.g., China) are said to have interdependent selves, whereas people in individualistic cultures (e.g., the United States) are said to have independent selves. To evaluate the effect of culture, we asked Chinese and American pairs to play a communication game that required perspective taking. Eye-gaze measures demonstrated that the Chinese participants were more tuned into their partner's perspective than were the American participants. Moreover, Americans often completely failed to take the perspective of their partner, whereas Chinese almost never did. We conclude that cultural patterns of interdependence focus attention on the other, causing Chinese to be better perspective takers than Americans. Although members of both cultures are able to distinguish between their perspective and another person's perspective, cultural patterns afford Chinese the effective use of this ability to interpret other people's actions. 相似文献
99.
100.
中学生知识获得过程是从情景记忆向语义记忆转化的过程 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
本研究探讨中学生的知识掌握过程。针对2001—2002年度上学期教学内容,对初中二年级学生的四门课程分别进行两次测验。对于每道选择题,学生首先选择正确答案,然后指出记忆的意识状态:记住的、知道的、猜测的。结果发现,学生记忆意识类型的转变(由记住向知道)取决于课程类型、保持时间和学生等级。对于不同课程,学生占优势的意识类型不同,自然学科知道的反应占优势,社会学科记住的反应占优势;优等生知道的反应多于其他学生;随着学习时间的延长,中学生的意识类型发生了由记住向知道的转变,这表明知识掌握过程中,中学生的知识表征发生了由情景记忆向语义记忆的转变。 相似文献