全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3940篇 |
免费 | 90篇 |
专业分类
4030篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 54篇 |
2019年 | 66篇 |
2018年 | 100篇 |
2017年 | 118篇 |
2016年 | 110篇 |
2015年 | 67篇 |
2014年 | 95篇 |
2013年 | 479篇 |
2012年 | 130篇 |
2011年 | 131篇 |
2010年 | 89篇 |
2009年 | 91篇 |
2008年 | 157篇 |
2007年 | 138篇 |
2006年 | 104篇 |
2005年 | 121篇 |
2004年 | 95篇 |
2003年 | 95篇 |
2002年 | 85篇 |
2001年 | 39篇 |
2000年 | 52篇 |
1999年 | 48篇 |
1998年 | 65篇 |
1997年 | 42篇 |
1996年 | 46篇 |
1995年 | 41篇 |
1994年 | 45篇 |
1992年 | 53篇 |
1990年 | 37篇 |
1989年 | 40篇 |
1988年 | 41篇 |
1987年 | 32篇 |
1986年 | 45篇 |
1985年 | 39篇 |
1984年 | 55篇 |
1983年 | 43篇 |
1982年 | 38篇 |
1981年 | 47篇 |
1980年 | 50篇 |
1979年 | 39篇 |
1978年 | 48篇 |
1977年 | 52篇 |
1976年 | 50篇 |
1975年 | 58篇 |
1974年 | 50篇 |
1973年 | 56篇 |
1972年 | 37篇 |
1971年 | 37篇 |
1970年 | 39篇 |
1968年 | 43篇 |
排序方式: 共有4030条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
In this study the stop signal task was employed to investigate inhibitory control in 15 children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 18 aggressive and 20 anxious children, and a group of 17 normal controls. The psychopathological groups were recruited from special educational services. Parent, teacher, and child questionnaires were used to select children with pervasive disorders. Controls attended regular classes and scored low on all questionnaires. Based on Quay's model of child psychopathology (Quay, 1988, 1993), we hypothesized a deficit in inhibitory control in children with externalizing disorders, whereas anxious children were predicted to be overinhibited. The ADHD group and the aggressive group showed poor inhibitory control and a slower inhibitory process. No evidence of overinhibition was found in anxious children. 相似文献
152.
Joseph Boyle 《Journal of Political Philosophy》2003,11(2):153-170
153.
A detailed analysis of a unique speech disturbance, marked by the frequent appearance in the speech stream of a meaningless intrusive syllable, is presented. Following a lengthy thoracic surgery, an American English speaking patient began to speak with non-English prosodic patterns, which evolved to a conspicuous intrusion in his speech of the syllable /sis/. This syllable and its variants were attached to words in a manner which conformed to the regular phonological rules in English (for formation of plural, possessive, and third person singular morphemes). The distribution and frequency of the intrusive syllable are described, and possible explanations for the abnormal occurrence of this particular syllable are discussed. 相似文献
154.
155.
Temporal integration of speech prosody is shaped by language experience: an fMRI study 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Gandour J Dzemidzic M Wong D Lowe M Tong Y Hsieh L Satthamnuwong N Lurito J 《Brain and language》2003,84(3):318-336
Differences in hemispheric functions underlying speech perception may be related to the size of temporal integration windows over which prosodic features (e.g., pitch) span in the speech signal. Chinese tone and intonation, both signaled by variations in pitch contours, span over shorter (local) and longer (global) temporal domains, respectively. This cross-linguistic (Chinese and English) study uses functional magnetic resonance imaging to show that pitch contours associated with tones are processed in the left hemisphere by Chinese listeners only, whereas pitch contours associated with intonation are processed predominantly in the right hemisphere. These findings argue against the view that all aspects of speech prosody are lateralized to the right hemisphere, and promote the idea that varying-sized temporal integration windows reflect a neurobiological adaptation to meet the 'prosodic needs' of a particular language. 相似文献
156.
Children in Grades K-5, selected for reading and behavior problems, received individual tutoring in a program which aimed to detail a hierarchy of reading skills, locate the point on the hierarchy at which each child should work, and provide enthusiastic social reinforcement for successes. Children were randomly assigned to higher or lower frequency tutoring (one 45-min. session every 1.6 days vs every 8.3 days). The higher frequency group progressed significantly faster in reading than the lower frequency group. Both groups progressed much faster during the time of the intervention than they had before tutoring. Before tutoring, both groups had progressed at about 0.5 grade per year; during tutoring, the higher frequency group progressed at 1.5 grade per year and the lower frequency group at 1.1 grade per year. The subsets of children with verbal ability scores one or two standard deviations below the population mean, as assessed on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, still progressed at average rates of 1.2 grade per year during tutoring. The amount of work students accomplished on a sounding and blending drill predicted reading progress. The intervention cost an average of 1,156 dollars per student per year. 相似文献
157.
Gail A Wasserman Pam Factor-Litvak Xinhua Liu Andrew C Todd Jennie K Kline Vesna Slavkovich Dusan Popovac Joseph H Graziano 《Child neuropsychology》2003,9(1):22-34
We report associations between serial measures of blood lead and intelligence in children age 10-12 years, half heavily exposed to lead from the prenatal period onward, and half relatively unexposed. For a subsample, we examine bone lead-IQ associations, comparing them with bone lead associations. Both blood and bone lead levels were associated with intelligence decrements, small relative to the contribution of social factors. For each doubling of Tib-Pb, Full Scale, Performance, and Verbal IQ decreased by an estimated 5.5, 6.2, and 4.1 points, respectively. Bone lead-IQ associations were stronger than those for blood lead, which nonetheless provide robust analogues. Current BPb, easy to obtain, provides a useful means for assessing Pb exposure/IQ associations. 相似文献
158.
Purpose
In the global economy, the need for understanding cross-cultural differences and the customer service-related processes involved in emotional labor is evident. The current study attempts to examine this issue by developing and testing hypotheses pertaining to cross-cultural differences between U.S. and Chinese service workers on the levels of display rule perceptions, emotion regulation, and burnout (i.e., emotional exhaustion, personal accomplishment, and depersonalization) as well as the relationships among these variables.Design/Methodology/Approach
Data was collected from service workers in the U.S. (n = 280) and China (n = 231). We tested for measurement differences, mean differences, and differences in the relationships among emotional labor variables between the two samples using a variety of analyses.Findings
It was found that the relatively robust sequence of display rules to surface acting to burnout was observed in a U.S. sample but was not observed in a Chinese sample, with some relationships being significantly weaker in the Chinese sample (e.g., surface acting to burnout dimensions) and others exhibiting relationships with the opposite sign (e.g., display rules were negatively related to surface acting in the Chinese sample).Implications
The results of this study suggest that many of the relationships among emotional labor variables vary as a function of the cultural context under consideration.Originality/Value
This is the first study to directly compare emotional labor across samples from Eastern and Western cultures. Additionally, this study begins to answer questions concerning why models of emotional labor generated in a Western culture may not apply in other cultures. 相似文献159.
Observers classified sine-wave and square-wave gratings on the basis of fundamental frequency (Are the bars wide or narrow?) or on the basis of higher harmonic frequencies (Are the bars sharp or fuzzy?). Stimuli were presented in either the left (LVF) or right (RVF) visual field. When the classification was made on the basis of the fundamental frequencies (1 or 3 c/deg), there was a LVF/right hemisphere advantage. However, when the classification was on the basis of a sharp/fuzzy distinction which involved searching for the higher harmonic frequencies, then a RVF/left hemisphere advantage was found. 相似文献
160.
Synaesthesia and eidetic imagery are both syncretic experiences entailing a dedifferentiation of perceptual qualities. In this paper the correlation between synaesthesia and eidetic imagery is explored. Ten subjects selected for possible eidetic and/or synaesthetic ability were tested in a battery of tasks that tap structural and typographic eidetic imagery, and colour-hearing and colour-mood synaesthesia. It was found that both structural and typographic eidetic imagery were correlated with measures of synaesthesia, indicating a relationship between the two phenomena. 相似文献