首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   186篇
  免费   8篇
  2023年   1篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   4篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   3篇
  2017年   7篇
  2016年   7篇
  2015年   9篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   10篇
  2012年   8篇
  2011年   17篇
  2010年   5篇
  2009年   9篇
  2008年   5篇
  2007年   9篇
  2006年   5篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   5篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   4篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   1篇
  1996年   1篇
  1993年   1篇
  1991年   1篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1974年   2篇
  1970年   2篇
  1968年   1篇
  1965年   2篇
  1964年   1篇
  1963年   2篇
  1962年   2篇
  1960年   2篇
排序方式: 共有194条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
151.
By means of amethod of ratio estimation, scale values were obtained for the subjective brightness of various physical intensities of monochromatic light of various wave lengths. In a second experiment the scale was constructed by a method of magnitude estimation. The brightness functions were studied by plotting the scale values against stimulus intensity for each wave length. The two experiments gave essentially the same results. It was shown: (1) Brightness of monochromatic light is a power function of stimulus intensity. The exponent of the function is approximately one-third for all wave lengths. (2) Properties of the brightness functions can explain certain empirical relations between brightness, hue and saturation.  相似文献   
152.
This study explores some of the psychological determinants of electrodermal responses to deception. There were four groups of 24 subjects, consisting of normal males and females, and personality-disordered males and females. The deceptive paradigms used consisted of three guilty knowledge card tasks that differed in the nature of the stimuli used and in procedural instructions. There was also a general question task involving five neutral questions and two emotive moral questions. After each task self-report analogue scales were administered in order to record the subjects' reactions to each task and the type of countermeasure they had used. The subjects also completed the Eysenck Personality Inventory, the Gough Socialization Scale and the Arrow-Dot test. Detectability on card tasks was found to relate to the following: procedural instructions, countermeasure strategies, involvement in the task, the extent to which the critical items are processed into memory, and lack of confidence in one's ability to beat the machine. Personality factors of Introversion, Neuroticism and Ego control had some significant correlations with differential responsivity but this was not consistent across subject samples or tasks. Differential responsivity to questions raising moral issues did not relate significantly to whether or not the subject admitted or denied such behaviour.  相似文献   
153.
The present study investigates the relationship between the frequency of aggressive behaviour of patients in a therapeutic community and socialization, extraversion and perceptual-motor rule-breaking. The subjects were 33 males and 24 females who had been rated by two observers in regard to frequency of aggressive behaviour during the initial part of their stay in the community. Principal component analysis revealed two fairly pure factors, referred to as verbal and physical aggression. In females both factors correlated significantly with extraversion, lack of role-taking ability and perceptual-motor rule-breaking. No significant correlations were obtained in the male sample except that physical aggression correlated positively with perceptual-motor rule-breaking.  相似文献   
154.
E isler , H. Empirical test of a model relating magnitude and category scales. Scand. J. Psychol ., 1962, 3 , 88–96.—The function K =α log( φ+ q/k) +β seems to describe the relation between category scale values K and subjective magnitudes φ. The additive constant q/k is obtained from the S ds of the magnitude estimates.
The model was empirically confirmed for the loudness and softness of white noise scaled by the methods of magnitude estimation and category rating.  相似文献   
155.
Coordination of Syntax and Semantics in Discourse Summary. 'Coordination' as used here is understood in the following way: Agents solve problems in on-going dialogue under mutual control and according to stable, well-understood patterns. In task-oriented dialogue the social frame for coordination is fixed. The need for coordination among agents arises because of differences in information, the dominant dialogue pattern 'directive - reply', and because of incompatibilities with respect to speakers' ontologies, language variation and agents' focus management. We discuss three dialogue examples showing coordination in some detail. In all of them the coordination problem is solved via a side sequence. Side sequences can be implemented either as autonomous dialogue contributions or they can be fused into the utterances they start from. Grammars treating 'syntax-in-dialogue' and, above all, side sequences, have to meet several constraints: They must describe various forms of extraposition to the right and long distance dependencies, produce and analyse by increments, and shift from production to reception and vice versa. All these patterns will be of relevance for the man-machine-interaction focused upon in the research unit “Situated Artificial Communicators“. It is suggested to set up the theory of grammar needed to accomplish all that within a theory of n-Person Cooperative Games. Zusammenfassung. Koordination im Dialog bedeutet, daß auftretende Probleme unter wechselseitiger Kontrolle der Agenten nach festgelegten Verfahren gelöst werden. In aufgabenorientierten Dialogen ist der soziale Rahmen für Koordination festgelegt. Koordination wird erforderlich wegen des Informationsgefälles zwischen Instrukteur und Konstrukteur, des dominanten Dialogmusters “Direktive geben“ - “Direktive befolgen“ sowie der Unterschiede in den Bereichen Sprecherontologie, Sprachvariation und Fokussteuerung. Wir geben drei Dialogbeispiele für Koordination, bei denen das Koordinationsproblem über eine Nebensequenz gelöst wird. Nebensequenzen, sog. “Verständigungssequenzen“, können als klar begrenzter, autonomer Subdialog realisiert oder in die Ausgangsäußerung eingefügt sein. Eine Syntax für den Dialog, insbesondere für Nebensequenzen und deren Einbettung, muß spezielle Konstruktionen wie Ausklammerung und Nachtrag erfassen, inkrementell operieren und zwischen Produktion und Rezeption hin- und herschalten können. Über alle diese Fähigkeiten sollte ein Situierter Künstlicher Kommunikator verfügen. Es wird vorgeschlagen, das dazu benötigte Grammatikmodell im Rahmen einer Theorie der Kooperativen Mehr-Personen-Spiele zu konzipieren.  相似文献   
156.
157.
Organizational ambidexterity has been established as an important antecedent of organizational innovation and performance. Recently, researchers have started to argue that ambidexterity is not only essential at the organizational, but also at the individual level. Thus, to be innovative, individuals need to engage in both explorative and exploitative behaviours. However, questions remain regarding the optimal balance of explorative and exploitative behaviours and how ambidexterity can be operationalized. At the organizational level, most empirical research utilized either the difference between, or the product of, exploration and exploitation. In this article, we criticize these approaches on conceptual and methodological grounds and argue for an alternative operationalization of ambidexterity: polynomial regression and response surface methodology. In two diary studies with daily and weekly data, we demonstrate the advantages of this approach. We discuss implications for ambidexterity research and innovation practice.  相似文献   
158.
Mendes N  Rakoczy H  Call J 《Cognition》2008,106(2):730-749
Developmental research suggests that whereas very young infants individuate objects purely on spatiotemporal grounds, from (at latest) around 1 year of age children are capable of individuating objects according to the kind they belong to and the properties they instantiate. As the latter ability has been found to correlate with language, some have speculated whether it might be essentially language dependent and therefore uniquely human. Existing studies with non-human primates seem to speak against this hypothesis, but fail to present conclusive evidence due to methodological shortcomings. In the present experiments we set out to test non-linguistic object individuation in three great ape species with a refined manual search methodology. Experiment 1 tested for spatiotemporal object individuation: Subjects saw 1 or 2 objects simultaneously being placed inside a box in which they could reach, and then in both conditions only found 1 object. After retrieval of the 1 object, subjects reached again significantly more often when they had seen 2 than when they had seen 1 object. Experiment 2 tested for object individuation according to property/kind information only: Subjects saw 1 object being placed inside the box, and then either found that object (expected) or an object of a different kind (unexpected). Analogously to Experiment 1, after retrieval of the 1 object, subjects reached again significantly more often in the unexpected than in the expected condition. These results thus confirm previous findings suggesting that individuating objects according to their property/kind is neither uniquely human nor essentially language dependent. It remains to be seen, however, whether this kind of object individuation requires sortal concepts as human linguistic thinkers use them, or whether some simpler form of tracking properties is sufficient.  相似文献   
159.
Mental state reasoning or theory-of-mind has been the subject of a rich body of imaging research. Although such investigations routinely tap a common set of regions, the precise function of each area remains a contentious matter. With the help of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we sought to determine which areas are involved when processing mental state or intentional metarepresentations by focusing on the relational aspect of such representations. Using non-intentional relational representations such as spatial relations between persons and between objects as a contrast, the results ascertained the involvement of the precuneus, the temporal poles, and the medial prefrontal cortex in the processing of intentional representations. In contrast, the anterior superior temporal sulcus and the left temporo-parietal junction were implicated when processing representations that refer to the presence of persons in relational contexts in general. The right temporo-parietal junction, however, was specifically activated for persons entering spatial relations. The level of representational complexity, a previously unexplored factor, was also found to modulate the neural response in some brain regions, such as the medial prefrontal cortex and the right temporo-parietal junction. These findings highlight the need to take into account the critical roles played by an extensive network of neural regions during mental state reasoning.  相似文献   
160.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号