首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   568篇
  免费   24篇
  592篇
  2023年   7篇
  2022年   8篇
  2021年   10篇
  2020年   19篇
  2019年   28篇
  2018年   24篇
  2017年   21篇
  2016年   28篇
  2015年   13篇
  2014年   19篇
  2013年   57篇
  2012年   31篇
  2011年   24篇
  2010年   20篇
  2009年   15篇
  2008年   31篇
  2007年   24篇
  2006年   26篇
  2005年   17篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   11篇
  2001年   10篇
  2000年   10篇
  1999年   9篇
  1998年   8篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   6篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   5篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   9篇
  1985年   7篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   2篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   2篇
  1976年   2篇
  1974年   1篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   2篇
  1965年   2篇
  1958年   1篇
排序方式: 共有592条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
We present some recent technical results of us on the incompleteness of classical analysis and then discuss our work on the Arnol'd decision problems for the stability of fixed points of dynamical systems.Partially supported by FAPESP and CNPq (Brazil, Philosophy Section).  相似文献   
112.
113.
Note critiche     
Giovanni Piana 《Axiomathes》1995,6(2):309-314

Critical notices
  相似文献   
114.
This study tests the hypothesis that the reported relationship between life event stress and physical illness is primarily a function of criterion and other content contamination in the stress measure. In particular, conventional life event measures include events related to physical health, which overlap with the criterion; events related to neuroticism, which influences the criterion; and events that are vague or subjective and could be affected by individual differences in psychological distress, response sets, and retrospective bias. In this study 386 adult males and females completed standard measures of life events and physical illness. Illness was significantly related to event subscales containing, respectively, health-related events, neuroticism-related events, and subjective events, but not to an "uncontaminated" event subscale. These results support the hypothesis of contamination and suggest that alternative approaches to the conceptualization and measurement of stress may need to be developed.  相似文献   
115.
Six measures of divergent thinking were administered to 825 men ranging in age from 17 to 101 over the period from 1959 to 1972; repeat administrations were given to a subset of 278 men after a 6-year interval. Cross-sectional analyses showed curvilinear trends, with an increase in scores for men under 40 and a decline thereafter. Repeated measures analyses on subjects initially aged 33 to 74 generally replicated this finding, whereas cross-sequential analyses suggested a decline for all cohorts tested at a later time. Additional analyses suggested that not all of the decline could be attributed to reduced speed of response production. These longitudinal findings confirm earlier cross-sectional reports of decline in divergent thinking abilities with age.  相似文献   
116.
117.
The study examined effects of instructional sets (objective, phenomenal, projective, and retinal) on the judged sizes of blocks placed at various “distances” in a pictorial array. Magnitude estimations of size were consistent with previous studies of size constancy in three-dimensional arrays. Chronometric analyses indicated that reaction time increased with distal size, but was not affected by perceived distance. The results suggested that size was scaled relative to a perceptual unit. Instructions affected the nature of the scale unit (proximal vs. distal), but not the scaling process itself.  相似文献   
118.
Two data sources--self-reports and peer ratings--and two instruments--adjective factors and questionnaire scales--were used to assess the five-factor model of personality. As in a previous study of self-reports (McCrae & Costa, 1985b), adjective factors of neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness-antagonism, and conscientiousness-undirectedness were identified in an analysis of 738 peer ratings of 275 adult subjects. Intraclass correlations among raters, ranging from .30 to .65, and correlations between mean peer ratings and self-reports, from .25 to .62, showed substantial cross-observer agreement on all five adjective factors. Similar results were seen in analyses of scales from the NEO Personality Inventory. Items from the adjective factors were used as guides in a discussion of the nature of the five factors. These data reinforce recent appeals for the adoption of the five-factor model in personality research and assessment.  相似文献   
119.
120.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号