全文获取类型
收费全文 | 240篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
242篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 2篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2016年 | 3篇 |
2015年 | 4篇 |
2014年 | 7篇 |
2013年 | 33篇 |
2012年 | 18篇 |
2011年 | 16篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 11篇 |
2007年 | 8篇 |
2006年 | 7篇 |
2005年 | 10篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 5篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1965年 | 3篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
1961年 | 1篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
1957年 | 3篇 |
1955年 | 2篇 |
1954年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有242条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
92.
Gilles Lemmel 《Psychologie Fran?aise》2004,49(1):51-62
This study examines school under-achievement from the perspective of clinical psychology. We compare 2 groups of children in the 3rd grade (mean age 9) based on their results on a national test of French language and Mathematics. We present the Rorschach results of 7 high performers compared to 7 low performers, all of whom have IQ scores (K. ABC) that are in the average range (90 - 109). The Rorschach data (Comprehensive System) demonstrate no differences between these groups of children concerning affective functioning and stress tolerance. However, the quantitative and qualitative results are notably different concerning attitudes relative to complex situations, in the area of critical self-perception and conflict-resolution strategies. 相似文献
93.
Simone Orlowski Sharon Lawn Gaston Antezana Anthony Venning Megan Winsall Niranjan Bidargaddi Ben Matthews 《Journal of child and family studies》2016,25(10):3066-3075
The imbalance between need and available resources with respect to youth mental ill-health has encouraged a growing body of literature around technology to support existing face-to-face services. However, this literature has not adequately investigated the perspective of youth as consumers and no data exists on the views of rural youth. In response to this gap, in-depth qualitative study investigated the perspectives of rural youth who were currently seeking help at a mental health service. Semi-structured interviews were carried out with a clinical sample of 10 young people (5 female), aged 16–22 years. Participants were recruited from two different mental health services located in two rural South Australian regions. Data were analysed via inductive thematic analysis. Results highlighted a young person’s desire for self-determination around their health and help-seeking within a service current environment that systematically subverts it. Overall, participants had long and complex histories of help-seeking associated with a history of isolation, disadvantage and trauma. A strong need for personal connection in the context of help-seeking was evident. Preferences for, and actual use of, the internet for mental healthcare existed on a continuum from no current (or future desire) to use technologies through to active interest in, and current use of, technologies as an adjunct to face-to-face care. Limited financial and infrastructural resourcing made it more difficult to access help online. Understanding and actively seeking out these views in design and implementation of technologies is in line with the current shift toward more consumer-focused and inclusive service design and delivery. 相似文献
94.
Paul C. Holinger M.Div. M.D. Granger E. Westberg D.D. 《Journal of religion and health》1975,14(1):14-21
Conclusion In conclusion, we have presented one small way in which the local church has potential to make a genuine contribution to healing of the sick and keeping the healthy healthy. We have discussed the rationale and present and projected models of church clinics. In the days to follow, we hope to establish and describe models that will deal still more effectively with the vast deficiencies, as seen in a variety of settings, in our health-care delivery system. Considering the tremendous resources of the local parish, we feel that involvement of that institution is most appropriate and are confident that such involvement will be healthy for both the practice of medicine and the mission of the church. 相似文献
95.
Einstein GO 《Memory & cognition》1976,4(6):701-708
Learning and retention were examined under varying amounts of intratask interference during learning. All subjects were required to learn the same list of auditorily presented words while concurrently processing a visually presented interfering list. Differential amounts of interference were produced by varying the relatedness of the interfering list to the learning list. The common learning list consisted of four general categories that also contained members of more restricted subcategories. Some subjects were instructed about the existence and names of the subcategories. The results showed that interference lengthened learning but, in some cases, facilitated retention relative to control groups. Restricted category knowledge facilitated learning but had no effect on retention. There were no significant retention differences after I week, but after 5 weeks retention performance was significantly better for groups that learned under related interference conditions. The results were interpreted in terms of more elaborate encoding of the items in the high intratask interference conditions. 相似文献
96.
Bonnie W. Camp Gaston E. Blom Frederick Hebert Willian J. van Doorninck 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1977,5(2):157-169
Think Aloud was designed as a training program to improve self-control in 6- to 8-year-old boys. It involved modeling and verbalization of cognitive activity to foster use of verbal mediation skills in dealing with both cognitive and interpersonal problems. It was hypothesized that this training would lead to improvement in test performance and teacher ratings of classroom behavior in hyperaggressive boys. Twelve aggressive second grade boys participated in daily, 30-minute, individual sessions for 6 weeks. Normal and aggressive control subjects received no intervention. Teachers rated both trained and untrained aggressive boys as improving in aggressive behaviors but they rated the experimental group as showing improvement on a significantly larger number of prosocial behaviors. The pattern of performance on cognitive tests also changed significantly in the experimental group. On pretest, their pattern differed from normals and resembled the aggressive control group, while on posttest their pattern resembled normals and differed from aggressive controls. Suggestions were made concerning additional refinements needed in the program, but overall results indicated potential value in the present approach for providing assistance to aggressive boys in the early grades.This investigation, from the Departments of Pediatrics and Psychiatry, University of Colorado Medical School, was supported in part by a Research Scientist Award No. MK2-47 356 from the National Institute of Mental Health and by Grant No. NEG 003-0029 from the National Institute of Education. However, the opinions expressed herein do not necessarily reflect the position or policy of the NIE, and no official endorsement by the National Institute of Education should be inferred.We are grateful to Mary Ann Bash and Margaret Simmons for their assistance in carrying out the program and to the Denver Public Schools for their continued cooperation. 相似文献
97.
Einstein GO McDaniel MA Thomas R Mayfield S Shank H Morrisette N Breneiser J 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》2005,134(3):327-342
Theoretically, prospective memory retrieval can be accomplished either by controlled monitoring of the environment for a target event or by a more reflexive process that spontaneously responds to the presence of a target event. These views were evaluated in Experiments 1-4 by examining whether performing a prospective memory task produced costs on the speed of performing the ongoing task. In Experiment 5, the authors directly tested for the existence of spontaneous retrieval. The results supported the multiprocess theory (M. A. McDaniel & G. O. Einstein, 2000) predictions that (a) spontaneous retrieval can occur and can support good prospective memory and (b) depending on task demands and individual differences, people rely to different degrees on monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval for prospective remembering. 相似文献
98.
Cortisol and Children’s Adjustment: The Moderating Role of Sympathetic Nervous System Activity 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
El-Sheikh M Erath SA Buckhalt JA Granger DA Mize J 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2008,36(4):601-611
We examined relations among cortisol, markers of sympathetic nervous system (SNS) activity (including salivary alpha-amylase and skin conductance level), and children's adjustment. We also tested the Bauer et al. (Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 23(2), 102-113, 2002) hypothesis that interactions between the SNS and cortisol would be associated with internalizing and externalizing problems. Saliva samples were obtained from 8- to 9-year-olds before and after a laboratory assessment battery, and were assayed for cortisol and alpha-amylase (sAA). Basal skin conductance level (SCL) was measured during resting conditions. Parents reported on child adjustment. Interactions between basal SNS and cortisol levels explained moderate amounts of unique variance in children's externalizing and internalizing problems. More specifically, higher basal cortisol levels were positively associated with higher internalizing and externalizing problems among children with higher SNS activity, as compared to children with lower SNS activity. Findings underscore the utility of including information about the coordination between hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) and SNS activity in biosocial models of atypical child development. 相似文献
99.
This paper focuses on two estimators of ability with logistic item response theory models: the Bayesian modal (BM) estimator
and the weighted likelihood (WL) estimator. For the BM estimator, Jeffreys’ prior distribution is considered, and the corresponding
estimator is referred to as the Jeffreys modal (JM) estimator. It is established that under the three-parameter logistic model,
the JM estimator returns larger estimates than the WL estimator. Several implications of this result are outlined. 相似文献
100.
We would like to propose a new model of meaning construction based on language comprehension considered as a dynamic process
during which the meaning of each linguistic unit and the global meaning of the sentence are determined simultaneously. This
model, which may be called “gestalt compositionality,” is radically opposed to the classic compositional mechanism advocated
by linguistic formalism based on the primacy of syntax. The process considers the syntactic structure of an utterance as the
product of meaning construction rather than its source. The comprehension of an utterance is consequently directly based on
the interaction between the different basic components of this utterance: lexical units, grammatical markers, positional relations
between units, and more generally, basic “constructions” in the sense of Construction Grammar. Thus, meaning is really the
result of a gestalt compositional process insomuch as the contribution of each basic component depends on the contribution
of the other components present in the utterance. We show a first attempt at modeling from French and English examples. 相似文献