全文获取类型
收费全文 | 139篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
142篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 1篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 1篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 2篇 |
2015年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 22篇 |
2012年 | 11篇 |
2011年 | 10篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 7篇 |
2008年 | 3篇 |
2007年 | 6篇 |
2006年 | 6篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有142条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Gilles Lemmel 《Psychologie Fran?aise》2004,49(1):51-62
This study examines school under-achievement from the perspective of clinical psychology. We compare 2 groups of children in the 3rd grade (mean age 9) based on their results on a national test of French language and Mathematics. We present the Rorschach results of 7 high performers compared to 7 low performers, all of whom have IQ scores (K. ABC) that are in the average range (90 - 109). The Rorschach data (Comprehensive System) demonstrate no differences between these groups of children concerning affective functioning and stress tolerance. However, the quantitative and qualitative results are notably different concerning attitudes relative to complex situations, in the area of critical self-perception and conflict-resolution strategies. 相似文献
52.
Gilles Dutilh Joachim Vandekerckhove Francis Tuerlinckx Eric-Jan Wagenmakers 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2009,16(6):1026-1036
When people repeatedly perform the same cognitive task, their mean response times (RTs) invariably decrease. The mathematical
function that best describes this decrease has been the subject of intense debate. Here, we seek a deeper understanding of
the practice effect by simultaneously taking into account the changes in accuracy and in RT distributions with practice, both
for correct and error responses. To this end, we used the Ratcliff diffusion model, a successful model of two-choice RTs that
decomposes the effect of practice into its constituent psychological processes. Analyses of data from a 10,000-trial lexical
decision task demonstrate that practice not only affects the speed of information processing, but also response caution, response
bias, and peripheral processing time. We conclude that the practice effect consists of multiple subcomponents, and that it
may be hazardous to abstract the interactive combination of these subcomponents in terms of a single output measure such as
mean RT for correct responses. Supplemental materials may be downloaded from http://pbr .psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental. 相似文献
53.
Pragmatic functions and mental spaces 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Gilles Fauconnier 《Cognition》1981,10(1-3):85-88
54.
Tom Loeys Catherine Legrand Antonio Schettino Gilles Pourtois 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》2014,67(2):304-327
The semi‐parametric proportional hazards model with crossed random effects has two important characteristics: it avoids explicit specification of the response time distribution by using semi‐parametric models, and it captures heterogeneity that is due to subjects and items. The proposed model has a proportionality parameter for the speed of each test taker, for the time intensity of each item, and for subject or item characteristics of interest. It is shown how all these parameters can be estimated by Markov chain Monte Carlo methods (Gibbs sampling). The performance of the estimation procedure is assessed with simulations and the model is further illustrated with the analysis of response times from a visual recognition task. 相似文献
55.
Perceptual and preferential decision making have been studied largely in isolation. Perceptual decisions are considered to be at a non–deliberative cognitive level and have an outside criterion that defines the quality of decisions. Preferential decisions are considered to be at a higher cognitive level and the quality of decisions depend on the decision maker’s subjective goals. Besides these crucial differences, both types of decisions also have in common that uncertain information about the choice situation has to be processed before a decision can be made. The present work aims to acknowledge the commonalities of both types of decision making to lay bare the crucial differences. For this aim we examine perceptual and preferential decisions with a novel choice paradigm that uses the identical stimulus material for both types of decisions. This paradigm allows us to model the decisions and response times of both types of decisions with the same sequential sampling model, the drift diffusion model. The results illustrate that the different incentive structure in both types of tasks changes people’s behavior so that they process information more efficiently and respond more cautiously in the perceptual as compared to the preferential task. These findings set out a perspective for further integration of perceptual and preferential decision making in a single ramework. 相似文献
56.
Einstein GO 《Memory & cognition》1976,4(6):701-708
Learning and retention were examined under varying amounts of intratask interference during learning. All subjects were required to learn the same list of auditorily presented words while concurrently processing a visually presented interfering list. Differential amounts of interference were produced by varying the relatedness of the interfering list to the learning list. The common learning list consisted of four general categories that also contained members of more restricted subcategories. Some subjects were instructed about the existence and names of the subcategories. The results showed that interference lengthened learning but, in some cases, facilitated retention relative to control groups. Restricted category knowledge facilitated learning but had no effect on retention. There were no significant retention differences after I week, but after 5 weeks retention performance was significantly better for groups that learned under related interference conditions. The results were interpreted in terms of more elaborate encoding of the items in the high intratask interference conditions. 相似文献
57.
Einstein GO McDaniel MA Thomas R Mayfield S Shank H Morrisette N Breneiser J 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》2005,134(3):327-342
Theoretically, prospective memory retrieval can be accomplished either by controlled monitoring of the environment for a target event or by a more reflexive process that spontaneously responds to the presence of a target event. These views were evaluated in Experiments 1-4 by examining whether performing a prospective memory task produced costs on the speed of performing the ongoing task. In Experiment 5, the authors directly tested for the existence of spontaneous retrieval. The results supported the multiprocess theory (M. A. McDaniel & G. O. Einstein, 2000) predictions that (a) spontaneous retrieval can occur and can support good prospective memory and (b) depending on task demands and individual differences, people rely to different degrees on monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval for prospective remembering. 相似文献
58.
A frequent assumption in the area of prospective memory is that a reminder to do an activity in the future improves the likelihood of doing the activity. The results of four experiments indicated, however, that the most general version of this assumption is incorrect. Subjects were either reminded of a prospective memory task several times during a retention interval or not reminded of the prospective memory task. The most effective reminders referred both to the prospective memory target events and to the intended activity. Reminders that referred only to the target events did not improve prospective memory (relative to a no-reminder control). Reminders that referred only to the intended activity did improve prospective memory, but not to the level of reminders that referred both to the target events and to the intended activity. Instructions to imagine oneself performing the prospective memory task did not further improve prospective memory. Neither the delay between the prospective memory instructions and the prospective memory cover task nor the delay between a reminder and a prospective memory target event significantly influenced performance. The results, which are discussed in terms of theoretical and practical implications, support a new theory of prospective memory and suggest surprising conditions under which reminders fail to benefit prospective memory. 相似文献
59.
This paper focuses on two estimators of ability with logistic item response theory models: the Bayesian modal (BM) estimator
and the weighted likelihood (WL) estimator. For the BM estimator, Jeffreys’ prior distribution is considered, and the corresponding
estimator is referred to as the Jeffreys modal (JM) estimator. It is established that under the three-parameter logistic model,
the JM estimator returns larger estimates than the WL estimator. Several implications of this result are outlined. 相似文献
60.
Peter G. Rendell Mark A. McDaniel Robert D. Forbes Gilles O. Einstein 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2013,20(3):236-256
ABSTRACT Two experiments examined the puzzling variation in the age-related patterns for event-based prospective memory tasks. Both experiments involved a famous faces ongoing task with a feature of the famous face as the target for the prospective memory task. In Experiment 1, a substantial age deficit was found on the prospective memory task when the cue was nonfocal (wearing glasses) to the ongoing task, replicating previous research, but this deficit was significantly reduced with a focal cue (first name John). In Experiment 2, the prospective memory cue (wearing glasses) was held constant and the demands of the ongoing task of naming faces were varied. The substantial age differences found with a nonfocal cue were eliminated when the ongoing task was made less challenging. The findings help reconcile the divergent age-related findings reported in the literature. 相似文献