首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   551篇
  免费   29篇
  2023年   6篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   8篇
  2020年   13篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   20篇
  2017年   15篇
  2016年   20篇
  2015年   8篇
  2014年   13篇
  2013年   91篇
  2012年   24篇
  2011年   19篇
  2010年   14篇
  2009年   10篇
  2008年   24篇
  2007年   16篇
  2006年   20篇
  2005年   15篇
  2004年   15篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   9篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   13篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   5篇
  1994年   11篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   5篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   7篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   10篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   5篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   3篇
  1974年   5篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   4篇
  1971年   5篇
  1970年   6篇
  1969年   5篇
  1968年   4篇
  1967年   9篇
  1966年   5篇
  1963年   4篇
  1962年   4篇
  1960年   4篇
排序方式: 共有580条查询结果,搜索用时 218 毫秒
141.
142.
143.
A solution for Case III of the Law of Comparative Judgment, modeled after Thurstone's solution for Case IV but eliminating the restrictive assumption of relatively equal discriminal dispersions, is developed.  相似文献   
144.
145.
146.
This study aimed to compare primary and secondary caregiver QOL within families of children with asthma and determine the potential importance of including secondary caregiver QOL in clinical and research settings. Participants included 118 families of children with asthma that had primary and secondary caregivers. Families completed measures in a single research session. Caregivers reported on QOL, psychological functioning, and family burden; children completed a measure of QOL. Child lung function was determined from objective spirometry. Adherence to prescribed controller medication was measured for 6 weeks following the research visit. Primary caregiver QOL was significantly lower than secondary caregiver QOL (Mean overall QOL of 5.85 versus 6.17, p < .05). Better medication adherence was associated with higher primary caregiver QOL (ρ = .22, p = .02); secondary caregiver QOL, not primary caregiver QOL, was positively associated with child QOL (ρ = .20, p = .03). Families with discrepant QOL scores between caregivers (difference in scores of at least .50) were characterized by more family burden and primary caregiver psychological symptoms. Differences in QOL scores between caregivers may be a reflection of primary caregivers’ greater investment in daily asthma management. In families reporting low burden and few psychological difficulties in the primary caregiver, QOL assessments from either caregiver may may be informative and representative of how parents are adapting to child asthma. In families experiencing high levels of burden or more primary caregiver psychological difficulties, QOL reports from secondary caregivers may not be as clinically meaningful.  相似文献   
147.
W. A. Gibson 《Psychometrika》1963,28(4):423-426
On the assumption that a partitioning can be found such that three mutually exclusive test vector configurations span the same factor space, a procedure is derived whereby symmetric parts of the correlation matrix are estimated from functions of asymmetric parts treated symmetrically. This yields an explicit matrix formula for communality estimation which generalizes earlier work by Albert. Conventional factoring methods, with all their computational and fitting advantages, can be applied once the symmetric portions of the correlation matrix have been estimated. Extension to four subgroups of test vectors allows for a matrix generalization of the old tetrad difference criterion to the multiple-factor case.  相似文献   
148.
Some big ideas for some big problems   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
Although most psychologists do not see sustainability as a psychological problem, our environmental predicament is caused largely by human behaviors, accompanied by relevant thoughts, feelings, attitudes, and values. The huge task of building sustainable cultures will require a great many psychologists from a variety of backgrounds. In an effort to stimulate the imaginations of a wide spectrum of psychologists to take on the crucial problem of sustainability, this article discusses 4 psychological approaches (neo-analytic, behavioral, social, and cognitive) and outlines some of their insights into environmentally relevant behavior. These models are useful for illuminating ways to increase environmentally responsible behaviors of clients, communities, and professional associations.  相似文献   
149.
150.
Inhibition of return and attentional control settings   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
In the present study, we used a spatial cuing paradigm in conjunction with a choice identification task to investigate whether exogenous attentional orienting and inhibition of return are affected by attentional control settings. As in previous studies (e.g., Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992), onset- and color-defined targets were crossed with uninformative onset- and color-defined cues. As expected, when the cue-target stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) was short (i.e., 100 msec), the results showed that exogenous attentional orienting was contingent on attentional set; attentional capture occurred in response to a particular cue only when the feature that defined the cue also defined the target (Folk et al., 1992). More importantly, when the cue-target SOA was long (i.e., 1,000 msec), the results showed that the occurrence of inhibition of return was also contingent on attentional set, at least partially so; inhibition of return occurred in response to onset cues only when they preceded onset targets. In contrast, inhibition of return never occurred in response to color cues (at a variety of long SOAs). The associations and dissociations that were observed between exogenous attentional orienting and inhibition of return are discussed in terms of posterior and anterior attention networks in the brain (Posner & Petersen, 1990).  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号