首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   369篇
  免费   16篇
  385篇
  2023年   3篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   13篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   9篇
  2017年   6篇
  2016年   7篇
  2015年   7篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   34篇
  2012年   16篇
  2011年   18篇
  2010年   12篇
  2009年   8篇
  2008年   19篇
  2007年   14篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   23篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   10篇
  2001年   10篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   7篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   8篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   4篇
  1989年   3篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   5篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   4篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   6篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   5篇
  1973年   4篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   4篇
  1969年   2篇
  1967年   2篇
  1966年   3篇
  1965年   2篇
排序方式: 共有385条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
Spatial models are employed to represent conceptual data in a wide range of fields within psychological research. In order to generate spatial models, it is necessary to first obtain empirical similarity data. A number of methods are available for collecting these data, but little effort has been made to compare their relative utility. In this article, we compare directly rated and five feature-based similarity data types in regard to their ability to be adequately represented by a spatial model (representational goodness of fit), and the ability of the representations to predict three external empirical variables (predictive validity). The results indicate that the representational goodness of fit of the feature-based similarities is noticeably superior to the directly rated similarities, and that the predictions of representations derived from common feature similarity data are substantially more likely than the predictions of all of the alternative representations. It is suggested that these findings are highly relevant to researchers employing spatial models to represent conceptual data, given that direct pairwise ratings have generally been considered the “gold standard” means of obtaining empirical similarities.  相似文献   
152.
Two experiments measured the joint influence of three key sets of semantic features on the frequency with which artifacts (Experiment 1) or plants and creatures (Experiment 2) were categorized in familiar categories. For artifacts, current function outweighed both originally intended function and current appearance. For biological kinds, appearance and behavior, an inner biological function, and appearance and behavior of offspring all had similarly strong effects on categorization. The data were analyzed to determine whether an independent cue model or an interactive model best accounted for how the effects of the three feature sets combined. Feature integration was found to be additive for artifacts but interactive for biological kinds. In keeping with this, membership in contrasting artifact categories tended to be superadditive, indicating overlapping categories, whereas for biological kinds, it was subadditive, indicating conceptual gaps between categories. It is argued that the results underline a key domain difference between artifact and biological concepts.  相似文献   
153.
Recent research at our laboratories in the field of human auditory time perception revealed that the duration of short empty time intervals (<~200 msec) is considerably underestimated if they are immediately preceded by shorter time intervals. Within a certain range, the amount of subjective time shrinking is a monotonous function of the preceding time interval: the shorter it is, the more it shrinks its successor. In the present study, the preceding interval was kept constant at 50 msec, and the following interval for which the duration had to be judged, varied from 40 to 280 msec. The results showed that at up to 100 msec, the perceived duration increased to a much lesser extent than did the objective duration. Beyond 120 msec, the perceived duration quickly increased and reached a veridical value at 160 msec. Such a sudden change of perceived duration in a temporal pattern in which the objective duration varies gradually indicates a typical example of categorical perception. We suggest that such a categorization of the time dimension might be a clue for processes of speech and music perception.  相似文献   
154.
Previous research has demonstrated that the subjective tempo of sequences of clicks that alternate between ears is slower than that of nonalternating sequences. Although the stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) between the clicks are the same in both conditions, their perceptual onset asynchronies (POAs) differ by 25 msec at all SOA values between 40 and 2,130 msec. It has been suggested that this subjective tempo difference originates only after a few clicks have been processed. The present study shows this not to be the case: The POA difference between interaural and monaural click sequences could also be established with sequences comprising only a few clicks.  相似文献   
155.
156.
157.
This paper proposes a consultative model for school psychology which includes several techniques that have been independently presented in the past. The primary contribution is the integration of these different methods into a model which is specifically designed for school psychologists. The proposed model is adapted from Caplan (1970), and it includes four levels of functioning: I) Direct Service to the Child; II) Indirect Service to the Child; III) Direct Service to the Teacher; and IV) Service to the School System; a distinction is made between the content and process of consultation, and a shift in emphasis from level I to the remaining levels is suggested.  相似文献   
158.
159.
160.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号