首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   21521篇
  免费   40篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   21篇
  2018年   3498篇
  2017年   2831篇
  2016年   2264篇
  2015年   206篇
  2014年   94篇
  2013年   211篇
  2012年   594篇
  2011年   2423篇
  2010年   2526篇
  2009年   1486篇
  2008年   1714篇
  2007年   2207篇
  2006年   55篇
  2005年   237篇
  2004年   189篇
  2003年   138篇
  2002年   90篇
  2001年   83篇
  2000年   95篇
  1999年   58篇
  1998年   40篇
  1997年   28篇
  1996年   12篇
  1995年   11篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   13篇
  1992年   12篇
  1991年   25篇
  1990年   34篇
  1989年   17篇
  1988年   14篇
  1987年   17篇
  1986年   18篇
  1985年   19篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   14篇
  1982年   13篇
  1980年   8篇
  1979年   11篇
  1978年   15篇
  1976年   13篇
  1975年   13篇
  1973年   15篇
  1972年   11篇
  1970年   13篇
  1969年   12篇
  1968年   14篇
  1967年   10篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
62.
63.
This paper reports on the reliability of children's responses on the Child and Adolescent Services Assessment (CASA) — a self-report instrument for use with 8– to 18-year-olds that gathers information about services used to address mental health problems. Findings were based on interviews completed by 77 children at a one week test-retest interval. Results showed that reports of lifetime service use were as reliable as were reports of service use in the preceding three months. Children reported restrictive and intrusive services more reliably than services that were provided in their natural environment. Reliability appeared to be associated more strongly with characteristics of the type of service than with characteristics of the child. Children also could report reliably on some details about their encounters with service providers (e.g., length of stay, number of visits, and onset of service use).  相似文献   
64.
Preface     
  相似文献   
65.
66.
Dubois  Cathy L. Z.  Knapp  Deborah E.  Faley  Robert H.  Kustis  Gary A. 《Sex roles》1998,39(9-10):731-749
This study examines sexual harassment (SH) whichinvolves members of the same gender, either male orfemale. Data are taken from the 1988 Department ofDefense Survey of Sex Roles in the Active Duty Military. Separate comparisons for male (38% White, 31%Black, and 31% ‘other’) and female (48%White, 27% Black, and 25% ‘other’) targetsare made between sameand other-gender SH related to fourmajor components of a conceptual model proposed by Fitzgerald,Drasgow, Hulin, Gelfand, & Magley (1997). Thesecomponents include sexual harassment behaviors, personalvulnerability, target response styles, and consequences of the SH for the target. The sexualorientation of targets and perpetrators is notconsidered because data were unavailable. Results reveala number of meaningful differences between sameandother-gender SH. The most striking result is that maletargets of same-gender SH experience consequences thatare significantly more pervasive and severe than thoseexperienced by male targets of other-gender SH.Organizational implications are discussed.  相似文献   
67.
68.
Semantic and geometric or physical similarity were manipulated separately in a backward-masking situation. When the target was a word to be read aloud, formal similarity between the letters of target and mask facilitated target recognition, as did associative similarity. Masking a target word by its own anagram also facilitated whole word report. In contrast, formal similarity was inhibitory rather than facilitatory of report when the target was spelled letter-by-letter, rather than read whole. This was true even for the same target words whose whole report was facilitated by formal similarity. A model to account for this reversal in the broader context of the neural substrate of reading is advanced. It is proposed that letter and word processing are fundamentally different in that letters are recognized by hierarchical feature analysis while words are stored and recognized wholistically by diffuse and redundant networks. Implications of the results for the study of reading are discussed.  相似文献   
69.
70.
A structural aspect of personal memories was examined in four studies. In some memories, one has the perspective of an observer, seeing oneself “from the outside.” In other memories, one sees the scene from one's own perspective; the field of view in such memories corresponds to that of the original situation. The existence of “observer” and “field” memories was confirmed in Study 1, using a recall questionnaire. In Study 2, the similarity structure of a specified set of eight to-be-recalled situations was established: the significant dimensions were “emotionality” and “self-awareness.” Study 3 related these dimensions to the observer-field distinction; situations involving a high degree of emotion and selfawareness were most likely to be recalled with an observer perspective. Recall set was varied in Study 4: a focus on feelings (as opposed to objective circumstances) produced relatively more field memories. Studies 3 and 4 also showed that events reported as more recent tend to be recalled in the field mode. Thus a qualitative characteristic of personal memories—the perspective from which they are experienced—is apparently related to characteristics of the original event, to the individual's purpose in recalling that event, and to the reported recall interval.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号