全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5318篇 |
免费 | 65篇 |
专业分类
5383篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 66篇 |
2019年 | 83篇 |
2018年 | 102篇 |
2017年 | 121篇 |
2016年 | 134篇 |
2015年 | 93篇 |
2014年 | 131篇 |
2013年 | 629篇 |
2012年 | 201篇 |
2011年 | 218篇 |
2010年 | 115篇 |
2009年 | 141篇 |
2008年 | 190篇 |
2007年 | 201篇 |
2006年 | 209篇 |
2005年 | 174篇 |
2004年 | 157篇 |
2003年 | 144篇 |
2002年 | 167篇 |
2001年 | 80篇 |
2000年 | 70篇 |
1999年 | 74篇 |
1998年 | 86篇 |
1997年 | 80篇 |
1996年 | 69篇 |
1995年 | 59篇 |
1994年 | 58篇 |
1993年 | 59篇 |
1992年 | 61篇 |
1991年 | 53篇 |
1990年 | 55篇 |
1989年 | 66篇 |
1988年 | 48篇 |
1987年 | 42篇 |
1986年 | 50篇 |
1985年 | 44篇 |
1984年 | 53篇 |
1983年 | 54篇 |
1982年 | 62篇 |
1981年 | 54篇 |
1980年 | 54篇 |
1979年 | 49篇 |
1978年 | 58篇 |
1977年 | 48篇 |
1976年 | 50篇 |
1975年 | 48篇 |
1974年 | 55篇 |
1973年 | 51篇 |
1971年 | 36篇 |
1969年 | 36篇 |
排序方式: 共有5383条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
The present experiment examined the shape of the attentional gradient in three-dimensional space. Subjects performed a response-compatibility task in which they were instructed to respond to a centrally located target and ignore flanking distractors. The irrelevant distractors were presented at combinations of seven different depths, three different horizontal separations, and three different vertical separations relative to the target. Depth was varied in a stereoscopic display viewed through polarized glasses. Overall, the size of the response-compatibility effect decreased with increased separation in all three dimensions. Interestingly, the response-compatibility effect was larger for horizontal separations than for vertical separations and was larger for crossed disparities than for uncrossed disparities. The results suggest an elliptical focus of attention, with steeper gradients in the vertical dimensions than in the horizontal dimensions. In addition, the results suggest, along the vertical dimension, a steeper gradient for objects located beyond the focus of attention relative to that for objects located between the observer and the focus of attention. 相似文献
122.
How do people go about choosing between alternatives in relatively simple settings? This study explores some of the variables that past work suggests may be relevant. Volunteer subjects worked for money in six procedures in which the probability of a payment from either of two alternatives was 1.0, but the rate of pay (i.e. the speed with which a payment was delivered or the size of the payment) interacted with the subjects recent allocation of choices, which we define as the ‘internalities’. Because of the internalities, choosing the currently more profitable alternative did not maximize total earnings. Subjects were more likely to fail to maximize when the interaction between present pay and past choices was spread over longer sequences of choices, or when the reward variable was the speed, rather than the value, of each payment. Subjects often disregarded the internalities and were instead guided by the current yields of the two alternatives, which is a frequently observed tendency, called ‘melioration’, in experiments on choices by animals. The tendency toward melioration was only partially counteracted by explicit instructions on how to maximize earnings. We discuss a theoretical framework for melioration that postulates both motivational and cognitive sources. 相似文献
123.
Despite the importance of probability assessment methods in behavioral decision theory and decision analysis, little attention has been directed at evaluating their reliability and validity. In fact, no comprehensive study of reliability has been undertaken. Since reliability is a necessary condition for validity, this oversight is significant. The present study was motivated by that oversight. We investigated the reliability of probability measures derived from three response modes: numerical probabilities, pie diagrams, and odds. Unlike previous studies, the experiment was designed to distinguish systematic deviations in probability judgments, such as those due to experience or practice, from random deviations. It was found that subjects assessed probabilities reliably for all three assessment methods regardless of the reliability measures employed. However, a small but statistically significant decrease over time in the magnitudes of assessed probabilities was observed. This effect was linked to a decrease in subjects overconfidence during the course of the experiment. 相似文献
124.
Lori J. Stark Lenora G. Knapp Anne M. Bowen Scott W. Powers Elissa Jelalian Sylvia Evans Mary Anne Passero Mary M. Mulvihill Mel Hovell 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1993,26(4):435-450
Three mildly malnourished children with cystic fibrosis and their parents participated in a behavioral group-treatment program that focused on promoting and maintaining increased calorie consumption. Treatment included nutritional education, gradually increasing calorie goals, contingency management, and relaxation training, and was evaluated in a multiple baseline design across four meals. Children's calorie intake increased across meals, and total calorie intake was 32% to 60% above baseline at posttreatment. Increased calorie consumption was maintained at the 96-week follow-up (2 years posttreatment). The children's growth rates in weight and height were greater during the 2 years following treatment than the year prior to treatment. Increases in pace of eating and calories consumed per minute were also observed 1 year posttreatment. These findings replicated and extended earlier research supporting the efficacy of behavioral intervention in the treatment of malnutrition in children with cystic fibrosis. 相似文献
125.
126.
Patient-Informed Treatment Development of Behavioral Smoking Cessation for People With Schizophrenia
Sarah M. Wilson Alexandra C. Thompson Emily D. Currence Shaun P. Thomas Eric A. Dedert Angela C. Kirby Eric B. Elbogen Scott D. Moore Patrick S. Calhoun Jean C. Beckham 《Behavior Therapy》2019,50(2):395-409
The objective of this study was to use qualitative methodology to tailor and refine an existing smoking cessation intervention for the population of people who use cigarettes and are diagnosed with schizophrenia, schizoaffective, or psychotic disorder. Successive cohort design methodology was used to iteratively modify the treatment in response to qualitative participant, therapist, and consultant feedback on the intervention. Qualitative methodology for participant feedback included analysis of semistructured interviews with participants, visualization of app utilization data, and stakeholder feedback from study therapists and consultants. Using the successive cohort design, a tailored multicomponent mobile health smoking cessation intervention was developed. The intervention included mobile contingency management (i.e., financial compensation for confirmed abstinence from smoking), pharmacotherapy for smoking cessation, cognitive-behavioral counseling sessions, and the Stay Quit app for relapse prevention. Two cohorts (N = 13) were completed in the study; after each cohort, the treatment protocol was revised. The intervention is described, as well as the qualitative findings from each cohort and subsequent changes made to the intervention based upon patient and provider feedback. Metrics of patient engagement included treatment adherence (40% in Cohort 1 and 63% in Cohort 2). Both participants and therapists reported that the intervention was helpful. Over one third of participants self-reported abstinence at posttreatment. Since qualitative methodology is often underutilized in mental health treatment development, this study demonstrates the utility of the successive cohort design for treatment development of behavior change interventions for at-risk, vulnerable populations. 相似文献
127.
Scott Normand 《Australasian journal of philosophy》2019,97(1):167-177
Ante rem structuralists claim that mathematical objects are places in ante rem structural universals. They also hold that the places in these structural universals instantiate themselves. This paper is an investigation of this self-instantiation thesis. I begin by pointing out that this thesis is of central importance: unless the places of a mathematical structure, such as the places of the natural number structure, themselves instantiate the structure, they cannot have any arithmetical properties. But if places do not have arithmetical properties, then they cannot be the natural numbers. The self-instantiation thesis turns out to be crucial for the identification of mathematical objects with places in structures. Unfortunately, we have no reason to believe that the self-instantiation thesis is true. 相似文献
128.
Identifying causal relationships is an important aspect of research and evaluation in visitor studies, such as making claims about the learning outcomes of a program or exhibit. Experimental and quasi-experimental approaches are powerful tools for addressing these causal questions. However, these designs are arguably underused in visitor studies. In this article, we offer examples of the use of experimental and quasi-experimental designs in science museums to aide investigators interested in expanding their methods toolkit and increasing their ability to make strong causal claims about programmatic experiences or relationships among variables. Using three designs from recent research (fully randomized experiment, posttest only quasi-experimental design with comparison condition, and posttest with independent pretest design), we discuss challenges and tradeoffs related to feasibility, participant experience, alignment with research questions, and internal and external validity. We end the article with broader reflections on the role of experimental and quasi-experimental designs in visitor studies. 相似文献
129.
Selim R. Benbadis Jeffrey R. Binder Sara J. Swanson Mariellen Fischer Thomas A. Hammeke George L. Morris Julie A. Frost Jane A. Springer 《Brain and language》1998,65(3):441-446
Background and objective: The intracarotid amobarbital procedure, or Wada test, is the method of choice to determine hemispheric representation of language, and is routinely used in the presurgical evaluation for intractable epilepsy. Some investigators perform comprehensive language assessments, but others base language lateralization solely on speech arrest. This study sought to determine whether speech arrest alone during Wada testing provides valid data regarding language lateralization. Methods: The subjects (previously reported) were 21 patients evaluated for intractable epilepsy, who underwent language lateralization by Wada testing and functional MRI (FMRI). For each patient, language representation was determined by calculating: (1) a Wada laterality index based exclusively on speech arrest; (2) a Wada laterality index based on comprehensive language assessment; and (3) an FMRI laterality quotient. Correlation coefficients and categorical classifications were analyzed. Results: There was no significant correlation between the Wada laterality quotient derived from duration of speech arrest and either the comprehensive Wada language laterality score (r= .35,p= .12) or FMRI language laterality score (r= .32,p= .16). Categorical classification as left, right or bilateral language also showed marked discordance between speech arrest and the other two methods. Conclusion: Duration of speech arrest during Wada testing is not a valid measure of language dominance. 相似文献
130.
Sophie K. Scott 《Psychological research》1998,61(1):4-11
P-centres are the subjective moments of occurrence of acoustic stimuli and capture properties of regularity and synchrony
in production and perception. Two experiments are described which compare the effects of onset and offset amplitude variation
and stimulus duration on P-centre location. The discussion is extended to consider the role of P-centres in cross modal temporal
phenomena.
Received: 14 November 1996 / Accepted: 23 March 1997 相似文献