全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4740篇 |
免费 | 67篇 |
专业分类
4807篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 72篇 |
2023年 | 34篇 |
2022年 | 56篇 |
2021年 | 75篇 |
2020年 | 141篇 |
2019年 | 108篇 |
2018年 | 206篇 |
2017年 | 204篇 |
2016年 | 233篇 |
2015年 | 174篇 |
2014年 | 194篇 |
2013年 | 653篇 |
2012年 | 245篇 |
2011年 | 241篇 |
2010年 | 157篇 |
2009年 | 158篇 |
2008年 | 222篇 |
2007年 | 215篇 |
2006年 | 167篇 |
2005年 | 107篇 |
2004年 | 112篇 |
2003年 | 124篇 |
2002年 | 101篇 |
2001年 | 80篇 |
2000年 | 99篇 |
1999年 | 51篇 |
1998年 | 42篇 |
1997年 | 36篇 |
1996年 | 32篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 20篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 14篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1975年 | 17篇 |
1974年 | 10篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
1971年 | 18篇 |
1970年 | 14篇 |
1966年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有4807条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
Summary The dependence of the subjective vertical (SV, the angle between a subjective vertical line and body median plane) on the gravity vertical (body tilt position, angle ) and on the optical vertical (i.e., a field of parallel lines seen as background to the line to be adjusted) was investigated. The SV was measured under dry and wet conditions at different degrees of body tilt attained in either clockwise (CW) or counterclockwise (CCW) progression.The measured difference in between field-of-lines left and right of the line is smallest at the upright position (=O°) and largest at =150°/165°. All body positions show a -difference between CW and CCW attainment (hysteresis), this too being least at upright and greatest at inverted body positions.These results, and changes of with test time, are discussed relative to the hypothesis that efficiency of the statolith organs decreases with body tilt increase, favouring increase of interference of somatoreceptors and the optical reference. 相似文献
162.
René A. De Wijk Wim Vaessen Johannes Heidema E. P. Köster 《Behavior research methods》1996,28(3):383-391
An injection olfactometer for human psychophysical experiments is described. The olfactometer is capable of presenting, either mono- or birhinally, single odorants and binary mixtures. The pulse form of the olfactory stimulus at the exit of the olfactometer approaches a rectangular form, which makes the olfactometer well suited for olfactory reaction time measurements. Since stimulation periods are independent of the subject’s inhalation, time-intensity measurements over prolonged periods of time are possible as well. In the second part of this paper, a new technique is described for the measurement of the stimulus pulse form at the exit of the olfactometer. 相似文献
163.
164.
Semantic categorization in the human brain: spatiotemporal dynamics revealed by magnetoencephalography 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Löw A Bentin S Rockstroh B Silberman Y Gomolla A Cohen R Elbert T 《Psychological science》2003,14(4):367-372
We examined the cortical representation of semantic categorization using magnetic source imaging in a task that revealed both dissociations among superordinate categories and associations among different base-level concepts within these categories. Around 200 ms after stimulus onset, the spatiotemporal correlation of brain activity elicited by base-level concepts was greater within than across superordinate categories in the right temporal lobe. Unsupervised clustering of data showed similar categorization between 210 and 450 ms mainly in the left hemisphere. This pattern suggests that well-defined semantic categories are represented in spatially distinct, macroscopically separable neural networks, independent of physical stimulus properties. In contrast, a broader, task-required categorization (natural/man-made) was not evident in our data. The perceptual dynamics of the categorization process is initially evident in the extrastriate areas of the right hemisphere; this activation is followed by higher-level activity along the ventral processing stream, implicating primarily the left temporal lobe. 相似文献
165.
Häyry M 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》2003,24(3):199-214
Are there distinctly European values in bioethics, and if there are, what are they? Some Continental philosophers have argued that the principles of dignity, precaution, and solidarity reflect the European ethos better than the liberal concepts of autonomy, harm, and justice. These principles, so the argument goes, elevate prudence over hedonism, communality over individualism, and moral sense over pragmatism. Contrary to what their proponents often believe, however, dignity, precaution, and solidarity can be interpreted in many ways, and it is not clear which reading would, or should, be favored by popular opinion. It is therefore dangerous to think that any one understanding of "European", or any other, values could be legitimately imposed on those who have different ideas about morality in health care and related fields. Bioethical principles should be employed to promote discussion, not to suppress it. 相似文献
166.
PD Dr. med. habil. Cornelia Albani Michael Geyer Horst Kächele Dan Pokorny 《Psychotherapeut》2003,48(6):388-402
Zusammenfassung Die Veränderung pathogenetisch bedeutsamer Beziehungsmuster gilt als wesentliches psychotherapeutisches Behandlungsziel. Um zielgerichtete Veränderungen von Beziehungsstrukturen zu erreichen, bedarf es sowohl einer Diagnostik und Beschreibung solcher Beziehungsmuster, einer darauf zielenden Behandlungstechnik als auch einer Verlaufskontrolle und Evaluation der angestrebten Veränderungen. Nach einem Überblick über verschiedene Methoden zur Erfassung von Beziehungsmustern wird die Methode des Zentralen-Beziehungskonflikt-Themas (ZBKT) von Luborsky näher beschrieben; ausgewählte Ergebnisse der ZBKT-Forschung werden dargestellt und die Methode kritisch bewertet.
相似文献
Cornelia AlbaniEmail: |
167.
Diedrichsen J Ivry RB Hazeltine E Kennerley S Cohen A 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2003,29(1):64-77
Four experiments were conducted to identify the locus of interference observed during the preparation of bimanual reaching movements. Target locations were specified by color, and the right-hand and left-hand targets could be either the same or a different color. Movements of different amplitudes (Experiment 1) or different directions (Experiment 2) to targets of the same color were initiated more quickly than symmetric movements to targets of different colors. These results indicate that costs observed during bimanual movements arise during target selection rather than during motor programming. Experiments 3 and 4 further examined the interference associated with target selection. Reaction time costs were found with unimanual movements when the target was presented among distractors associated with responses for the other hand. Interference observed during bimanual reaching appears to reflect difficulty in segregating the response rules assigned to each hand. 相似文献
168.
Cross-sectional differences and longitudinal changes in cognitive functioning in relation to mortality across a 7-year follow-up period, with 3 times of measurement, were examined in a population-based sample of very old adults. The authors also sought to determine whether cause of death (cerebro/cardiovascular disease [CVD]; non-CVD) modified the magnitude of mortality-related cognitive deficits. Cognitive performance was indexed by tests of general cognitive ability, episodic memory, primary memory, verbal fluency, and visuospatial ability. Results indicated cross-sectional differences on all domains of functioning, with persons who would die within 3 years after baseline testing performing more poorly. Longitudinally, greater decrements were observed on all domains for persons who would die after the first follow-up period, as compared with survivors. Cause of death failed to modify the magnitude of the cross-sectional and longitudinal deficits. The pattern of results point to the general nature of this phenomenon. 相似文献
169.
Hietanen JK Leppänen JM 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2003,29(6):1228-1243
In 6 experiments, the authors investigated whether attention orienting by gaze direction is modulated by the emotional expression (neutral, happy, angry, or fearful) on the face. The results showed a clear spatial cuing effect by gaze direction but no effect by facial expression. In addition, it was shown that the cuing effect was stronger with schematic faces than with real faces, that gaze cuing could be achieved at very short stimulus onset asynchronies (14 ms), and that there was no evidence for a difference in the strength of cuing triggered by static gaze cues and by cues involving apparent motion of the pupils. In sum, the results suggest that in normal, healthy adults, eye direction processing for attention shifts is independent of facial expression analysis. 相似文献
170.