全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1188篇 |
免费 | 41篇 |
专业分类
1229篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 15篇 |
2018年 | 34篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 51篇 |
2015年 | 17篇 |
2014年 | 36篇 |
2013年 | 111篇 |
2012年 | 54篇 |
2011年 | 68篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 45篇 |
2008年 | 53篇 |
2007年 | 53篇 |
2006年 | 41篇 |
2005年 | 39篇 |
2004年 | 45篇 |
2003年 | 45篇 |
2002年 | 34篇 |
2001年 | 19篇 |
2000年 | 23篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 23篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 17篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1229条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
We propose that diversity is a malleable concept capable of being used either to attenuate or to enhance racial inequality. The research reported here suggests that when people are exposed to ambiguous information concerning an organization's diversity, they construe diversity in a manner consistent with their social-dominance motives. Specifically, anti-egalitarian individuals broaden their construal of diversity to include nonracial (i.e., occupational) heterogeneity when an organization's racial heterogeneity is low. By contrast, egalitarian individuals broaden their construal of diversity to include nonracial heterogeneity when an organization's racial heterogeneity is high. The inclusion of occupational heterogeneity in perceptions of diversity allows people across the spectrum of social-dominance orientation to justify their support for or opposition to hierarchy-attenuating affirmative-action policies. Our findings suggest that diversity may not have a fixed meaning and that, without a specific delineation of what the concept means in particular contexts, people may construe diversity in a manner consistent with their social motivations. 相似文献
142.
DC Palmer 《The Behavior analyst / MABA》2012,35(1):59-73
Evolution and reinforcement shape adaptive forms and adaptive behavior through many cycles of blind variation and selection, and therein lie their parsimony and power. Human behavior is distinctive in that this shaping process is commonly "short circuited": Critical variations are induced in a single trial. The processes by which this economy is accomplished have a common feature: They all exploit one or more atomic repertoires, elementary units of behavior each under control of a distinctive stimulus. By appropriate arrangements of these discriminative stimuli, an indefinite number of permutations of atomic units can be evoked. When such a permutation satisfies a second contingency, it can come under control of the relevant context, and the explicit arrangement of discriminative stimuli will no longer be required. Consequently, innovations in adaptive behavior can spread rapidly through the population. A consideration of atomic repertoires informs our interpretation of generalized operants and other phenomena that are otherwise difficult to explain. Observational learning is discussed as a case in point. 相似文献
143.
Delaney M Leroi I Simpson J Overton PG 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2012,19(3):338-346
Parkinson's disease (PD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disease primarily characterised by motor symptoms. However, another feature of PD which is receiving increasing attention is the phenomenon of impulse control disorders (ICDs), such as pathological gambling. To date, research into ICDs in PD has centred on a biomedical model of cause, related to the effects of dopamine replacement therapy. However, there are several areas of discrepancy in the current biomedical account of ICDs in PD. In addition, we argue that social and psychological factors also need to be considered to achieve a more complete understanding of the phenomenon. We present a novel conceptual model which combines biomedical and psychosocial factors in the genesis of ICDs in PD and use the model to identify a number of potential treatment intervention points and to highlight important outstanding questions concerning the inter-relationship between psychosocial and biomedical factors in the genesis of ICDs in PD. 相似文献
144.
Anne‐Lise Goddings Stephanie Burnett Heyes Geoffrey Bird Russell M. Viner Sarah‐Jayne Blakemore 《Developmental science》2012,15(6):801-811
The social brain undergoes developmental change during adolescence, and pubertal hormones are hypothesized to contribute to this development. We used fMRI to explore how pubertal indicators (salivary concentrations of testosterone, oestradiol and DHEA; pubertal stage; menarcheal status) relate to brain activity during a social emotion task. Forty‐two females aged 11.1 to 13.7 years underwent fMRI scanning while reading scenarios pertaining either to social emotions, which require the representation of another person’s mental states, or to basic emotions, which do not. Pubertal stage and menarcheal status were used to assign girls to early or late puberty groups. Across the entire sample, the contrast between social versus basic emotion resulted in activity within the social brain network, including dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), the posterior superior temporal sulcus, and the anterior temporal cortex (ATC) in both hemispheres. Increased hormone levels (independent of age) were associated with higher left ATC activity during social emotion processing. More advanced age (independent of hormone levels) was associated with lower DMPFC activity during social emotion processing. Our results suggest functionally dissociable effects of pubertal hormones and age on the adolescent social brain. 相似文献
145.
146.
Arita JT Carlisle NB Woodman GF 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2012,38(3):580-584
Theories of attention are compatible with the idea that we can bias attention to avoid selecting objects that have known nontarget features. Although this may underlie several existing phenomena, the explicit guidance of attention away from known nontargets has yet to be demonstrated. Here we show that observers can use feature cues (i.e., color) to bias attention away from nontarget items during visual search. These negative cues were used to quickly instantiate a template for rejection that reliably facilitated search across the cue-to-search stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs), although negative cues were not as potent as cues that guide attention toward target features. Furthermore, by varying the search set size we show a template for rejection is increasingly effective in facilitating search as scene complexity increases. Our findings demonstrate that knowing what not to look for can be used to configure attention to avoid certain features, complimenting what is known about setting attention to select certain target features. 相似文献
147.
Marta Gil Michelle Symonds Geoffrey Hall Isabel de Brugada 《Learning and motivation》2011,42(4):273-281
In three experiments, rats received exposure to a sucrose solution followed by conditioning with a neutral flavor as the conditioned stimulus (CS) and sucrose as the unconditioned stimulus (US). In Experiments 1 and 2, some rats were given both the preexposure and the conditioning phases in a highly familiar context (the homecage), whereas other animals received both phases in a novel and distinctive context. In both cases the magnitude of the conditioning effect was reduced by preexposure to the US. Experiment 3 directly assessed the possible role of contextual cues by changing the context between the exposure phase and the conditioning phase but found no loss of the US-preexposure effect in these conditions. These results lend no support to the blocking-by-context account of the US-preexposure effect; alternative interpretations are considered. 相似文献
148.
Extending the simultaneous-sequential paradigm to measure perceptual capacity for features and words
Scharff A Palmer J Moore CM 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2011,37(3):813-833
In perception, divided attention refers to conditions in which multiple stimuli are relevant to an observer. To measure the effect of divided attention in terms of perceptual capacity, we introduce an extension of the simultaneous-sequential paradigm. The extension makes predictions for fixed-capacity models as well as for unlimited-capacity models. We apply this paradigm to two example tasks, contrast discrimination and word categorization, and find dramatically different effects of divided attention. Contrast discrimination has unlimited capacity, consistent with independent, parallel processing. Word categorization has a nearly fixed capacity, consistent with either serial processing or fixed-capacity, parallel processing. We argue that these measures of perceptual capacity rely on relatively few assumptions compared to most alternative measures. 相似文献
149.
150.