首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   894篇
  免费   60篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   17篇
  2017年   25篇
  2016年   36篇
  2015年   9篇
  2014年   20篇
  2013年   91篇
  2012年   43篇
  2011年   50篇
  2010年   24篇
  2009年   43篇
  2008年   52篇
  2007年   42篇
  2006年   31篇
  2005年   34篇
  2004年   38篇
  2003年   37篇
  2002年   28篇
  2001年   19篇
  2000年   11篇
  1999年   15篇
  1998年   17篇
  1997年   11篇
  1996年   9篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   13篇
  1993年   18篇
  1992年   15篇
  1991年   11篇
  1990年   6篇
  1989年   9篇
  1988年   10篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   15篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   9篇
  1982年   16篇
  1981年   10篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   10篇
  1978年   12篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   10篇
  1973年   4篇
  1972年   4篇
排序方式: 共有955条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
901.
We investigated 3-year-olds’ and adults’ use of domain cues in learning words for solid and nonsolid material entities. In Experiments 1 and 2, participants heard a novel neutral noun (e.g., “my X”) for a standard solid or nonsolid entity described as either a toy or a food. They then were asked to extend the word to one of two other entities. Both options matched the standard in solidity; but one differed from it in an object-relevant property (shape) and the other in a substance-relevant property (color, texture, or smell). Both children and adults were more likely to select the same-shaped entity if the standard was (1) solid than if it was nonsolid, and (2) described as a toy than if it was described as a food. Their interpretations of novel words for material entities were thus affected not only by perceptual information (about solidity) but also by conceptual information (about domain). In Experiment 3, the novel noun was presented in a syntactic context that suggested the solid entity should be interpreted as an object (e.g., “an X”) and that the nonsolid entity should be interpreted as a substance (e.g., “some X”). For adults, these changes largely eliminated the effect of the entity’s domain (toy, food) on interpretation. We interpret these findings in terms of the proposal that domain cues, like solidity cues, furnish information about whether an entity’s structure should be thought of as arbitrary or nonarbitrary and, hence, about whether a word should be interpreted as naming an object or a substance construal.  相似文献   
902.
Ni, Crain, and Shankweiler (1996) present evidence to suggest that the focus operator only can guide how reduced relative clause sentences are initially parsed. In this paper, we demonstrate that this does not hold for relative clause sentences that start with a noun-phrase, verb, noun-phrase construction. We report an eye movement study in which subjects read reduced and unreduced sentences of this type with and without the focus operator only. There were longer first-pass reading times in the critical region of reduced sentences than in the same region of unreduced sentences, regardless of the inclusion of only. Furthermore, readers spent less time re-inspecting portions of text after being garden pathed when reading reduced relative clause sentences that contained the focus operator than when reading reduced relative clause sentences that did not. We conclude that subjects initially syntactically misanalysed reduced relative clause sentences with and without only, and the inclusion of a focus operator facilitated recovery procedures rather than guiding initial parsing. These results are inconsistent with the referential theory and undermine the conclusions of Ni et al. (1996).  相似文献   
903.
904.
Associations between the satisfaction of basic psychological needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness with current suicidal ideation and risk for suicidal behavior were examined. Two logistic regressions were conducted with a cross‐sectional database of 440 university students to examine the association of need satisfaction with suicidal ideation and risk for suicidal behavior, while controlling for demographics and depressive symptoms. Suicidal ideation was reported by 15% of participants and 18% were found to be at risk for suicidal behavior. A one standard deviation increase in need satisfaction reduced the odds of suicidal ideation by 53%, OR (95% CI) = 0.47 (0.33–0.67), and the odds of being at risk for suicidal behavior by 50%, OR (95% CI) = 0.50 (0.37–0.69). Young adults whose basic psychological needs are met may be less likely to consider suicide and engage in suicidal behavior. Prospective research is needed to confirm these associations.  相似文献   
905.
906.
Six experiments examined the determinants of the numeral advantage effect: the finding that memory span for Arabic numerals (1, 2, 3, etc.) is greater than for digit words (one, two, three, etc.). The speed of item identification for numeral and digit words was unrelated to memory span for the same items and a larger memory span for numerals persisted under concurrent random generation (Experiment 1). The numeral advantage, however, was abolished when the items were presented in random locations within an invisible 3x3 grid (Experiment 2) and in locations on a horizontal plane that ran contrary to the natural direction of reading (Experiment 3). When the items were presented in the same location, a disruption of the spatial component of visuo-spatial working memory eliminated the numeral advantage (Experiment 4), whereas interference with the visual component of the system did not (Experiment 5). When the items were spatially distributed in a 3x3 matrix, however, neither visual nor spatial interference abolished the effect (Experiment 6). Taken together, these findings suggest that the numeral advantage effect is mediated by discrete components in visuo-spatial working memory dedicated to the temporary storage and renewal of visual codes and questions the assumption that the underlying mechanisms in immediate, visual serial recall are equivalent between stimulus categories.  相似文献   
907.
908.
In three experiments, pigeons with lesions of the paleostriatum (experimental subjects) and unoperated control birds were trained on tasks designed to assess their instrumental learning abilities. In Experiment 1, using an orthodox Skinner box, training was given on a variable interval (VI) followed by a fixed interval (FI) schedule of reinforcement and only non-significant differences between the groups emerged. Experiment 2 examined the performance of the same subjects on a VI schedule in which a response-contingent signal accompanied reinforcement. For control subjects the presence of the signal resulted in a low rate of response compared with that found in equivalent conditions with the signal omitted. Experimental subjects showed the same response rate when the signal was present as when it was absent. Experiment 3 employed naive subjects, and a Skinner box modified to facilitate key-pecking in total darkness. In this apparatus, experimental subjects showed a lowered response rate on a VI schedule. These tasks were analyzed in terms of the classical (stimulus-reinforcer) and instrumental (response-reinforcer) learning they involve. The results suggest that pigeons with paleostriatal lesions show a deficit in forming response-reinforcer associations, perhaps because the lesions reduce the salience of response-produced cues.  相似文献   
909.
Book reviews     
DONAHOE, J. W. and WESSELS, M. G. Learning, Language and Memory. New York: Harper & Row. 1980. Pp. 513.

GRIFFIN, D. R. The Question of Animal Awareness: Evolutionary Continuity of Mental Experience. (Revised and enlarged edition.) New York: The Rockefeller University Press. 1981. Pp. xi + 209. $13.95. ISBN 87470 035 3.  相似文献   
910.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号