首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3473篇
  免费   190篇
  2023年   25篇
  2022年   29篇
  2021年   47篇
  2020年   71篇
  2019年   84篇
  2018年   87篇
  2017年   111篇
  2016年   121篇
  2015年   83篇
  2014年   88篇
  2013年   411篇
  2012年   157篇
  2011年   138篇
  2010年   101篇
  2009年   93篇
  2008年   125篇
  2007年   109篇
  2006年   109篇
  2005年   96篇
  2004年   82篇
  2003年   96篇
  2002年   89篇
  2001年   59篇
  2000年   82篇
  1999年   60篇
  1998年   61篇
  1997年   44篇
  1996年   39篇
  1995年   26篇
  1994年   28篇
  1993年   34篇
  1992年   43篇
  1991年   49篇
  1990年   28篇
  1989年   41篇
  1988年   25篇
  1987年   31篇
  1986年   28篇
  1985年   39篇
  1984年   23篇
  1982年   31篇
  1981年   34篇
  1980年   32篇
  1979年   30篇
  1978年   28篇
  1975年   35篇
  1974年   37篇
  1973年   32篇
  1972年   24篇
  1971年   24篇
排序方式: 共有3663条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
152.
153.
The Psychological Record - Creativity and innovation have brought about phenomenal changes throughout human history. Art, science, medicine, industry, and technology have all grown through creative...  相似文献   
154.
Pietri  Evava S.  Johnson  India R.  Majid  Sana  Chu  Charles 《Sex roles》2021,84(1-2):14-33
Sex Roles - Although interactions with Black female scientists can alter beliefs about STEM (science, technology, engineering, mathematics) and encourage Black female students’ interest in...  相似文献   
155.
Topological relations such as inside, outside, or intersection are ubiquitous to our spatial thinking. Here, we examined how people reason deductively with topological relations between points, lines, and circles in geometric diagrams. We hypothesized in particular that a counterexample search generally underlies this type of reasoning. We first verified that educated adults without specific math training were able to produce correct diagrammatic representations contained in the premisses of an inference. Our first experiment then revealed that subjects who correctly judged an inference as invalid almost always produced a counterexample to support their answer. Noticeably, even if the counterexample always bore a certain level of similarity to the initial diagram, we observed that an object was more likely to be varied between the two drawings if it was present in the conclusion of the inference. Experiments 2 and 3 then directly probed counterexample search. While participants were asked to evaluate a conclusion on the basis of a given diagram and some premisses, we modulated the difficulty of reaching a counterexample from the diagram. Our results indicate that both decreasing the counterexample density and increasing the counterexample distance impaired reasoning performance. Taken together, our results suggest that a search procedure for counterexamples, which proceeds object-wise, could underlie diagram-based geometric reasoning. Transposing points, lines, and circles to our spatial environment, the present study may ultimately provide insights on how humans reason about topological relations between positions, paths, and regions.  相似文献   
156.
157.
158.
This article traces the origins of language about ‘theories’ of the atonement in Friedrich Schleiermacher and F.C. Baur’s historicization of Protestant theology in Germany. It follows the growth and promulgation of this conceptualization as it spread to America and Great Britain in the later nineteenth century. This approach contributed to the growth and development of the doctrine and its history, while severely limiting it in important ways. I note four dangers implicit within uncritical use of ‘theory’ language in studies of the history of the doctrine of the atonement, and conclude by arguing that a more fitting approach lies in the ancient and medieval concept of theoria, or contemplation, which serves as the broader telos for appreciating the riches we have in the work of Christ, within which modest contemporary notions of theory can and should play a role.  相似文献   
159.
160.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号