全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1818篇 |
免费 | 16篇 |
专业分类
1834篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 58篇 |
2019年 | 50篇 |
2018年 | 25篇 |
2017年 | 27篇 |
2016年 | 49篇 |
2015年 | 52篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 152篇 |
2012年 | 67篇 |
2011年 | 55篇 |
2010年 | 39篇 |
2009年 | 39篇 |
2008年 | 58篇 |
2007年 | 74篇 |
2006年 | 56篇 |
2005年 | 60篇 |
2004年 | 71篇 |
2003年 | 65篇 |
2002年 | 53篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 19篇 |
1999年 | 24篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 26篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 25篇 |
1993年 | 27篇 |
1992年 | 22篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 33篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 20篇 |
1984年 | 27篇 |
1983年 | 21篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 27篇 |
1980年 | 25篇 |
1979年 | 29篇 |
1978年 | 32篇 |
1977年 | 19篇 |
1976年 | 22篇 |
1975年 | 22篇 |
1974年 | 27篇 |
1973年 | 17篇 |
1972年 | 17篇 |
1971年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有1834条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
831.
Research on the association of personality to political orientation has suggested that direct influences are modest. Here we used a personality system model in which direct influences on political behaviour flow from moral values, with personality mostly acting on these characteristic moral adaptations, rather than directly affecting political attitudes. Study 1 in 447 subjects supported this model, with significant effects on political orientation flowing from four of the five-factor model personality domains, but largely mediated through moral values concerning the importance of group order and individual rights. This personality system model was replicated in an independent study (n= 476) using a US sample and including a different measure of politics. Both studies support predictions that personality has significant effects on political attitudes, but that these are exerted largely via moral values. These findings help to explain inconsistencies in previous studies attempting to link personality to political orientation that have not included the intermediary level of values. 相似文献
832.
Stephanie J. Sohl Julie B. Schnur Madalina Sucala Daniel David Gary Winkel Guy H. Montgomery 《Psychology & health》2013,28(3):347-361
Understanding precursors to distress and emotional well-being (EWB) experienced in anticipation of radiotherapy would facilitate the ability to intervene with this emotional upset (i.e. higher distress, lower EWB). Thus, this study tested an expectancy-based model for explaining emotional upset in breast cancer patients prior to radiotherapy. Women affected by breast cancer (N?=?106) were recruited and participants completed questionnaires prior to commencing radiotherapy. Structural equation modelling was used to test a cross-sectional model, which assessed the ability of dispositional optimism (Life Orientation Test-Revised – two factors), response expectancies (Visual Analog Scale items), medical (type of surgery, cancer stage and chemotherapy history) and demographic (age, race, ethnicity, education and marital status) variables to predict both EWB (Functional Assessment of Chronic Illness Therapy – Emotional Well-being Subscale) and distress (Profile of Mood States – short version). The model represented a good fit to the data accounting for 65% of the variance in EWB and 69% in distress. Significant predictors of emotional upset were pessimism, response expectancies, Latina ethnicity, cancer stage and having had a mastectomy. These variables explained a large portion of emotional upset experienced prior to radiotherapy for breast cancer and are important to consider when aiming to reduce distress and improve EWB in this context. 相似文献
833.
834.
Gary Ebbs 《逻辑史和逻辑哲学》2013,34(4):346-366
The Hilbert–Bernays Theorem establishes that for any satisfiable first-order quantificational schema S, one can write out linguistic expressions that are guaranteed to yield a true sentence of elementary arithmetic when they are substituted for the predicate letters in S. The theorem implies that if L is a consistent, fully interpreted language rich enough to express elementary arithmetic, then a schema S is valid if and only if every sentence of L that can be obtained by substituting predicates of L for predicate letters in S is true. The theorem therefore licenses us to define validity substitutionally in languages rich enough to express arithmetic. The heart of the theorem is an arithmetization of Gödel's completeness proof for first-order predicate logic. Hilbert and Bernays were the first to prove that there is such an arithmetization. Kleene established a strengthened version of it, and Kreisel, Mostowski, and Putnam refined Kleene's result. Despite the later refinements, Kleene's presentation of the arithmetization is still regarded as the standard one. It is highly compressed, however, and very difficult to read. My goals in this paper are expository: to present the basics of Kleene's arithmetization in a less compressed, more easily readable form, in a setting that highlights its relevance to issues in the philosophy of logic, especially to Quine's substitutional definition of logical truth, and to formulate the Hilbert–Bernays Theorem in a way that incorporates Kreisel's, Mostowski's, and Putnam's refinements of Kleene's result. 相似文献
835.
836.
We measured the effects of divided attention on the aggressive behavior of a 20‐year‐old man within a test‐control functional analysis (FA; Iwata, B. A., Duncan, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Lerman, D. C., & Shore, B. A. (1994). A sequential, test‐control methodology for conducting functional analyses of self‐injurious behavior. Behavior Modification, 18, 289–306). The FA confirmed that divided attention was associated with high‐frequency aggression and aggression‐contingent attention from a familiar and preferred staff person. A subsequent treatment evaluation confirmed that aggression decreased when the man was able to request attention from the staff person under the divided attention condition. We discuss the clinical utility of a divided attention and test‐control methodology when conducting a FA of serious problem behavior within applied settings Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
837.
E. Layne Paddock Jaewon Ko Russell Cropanzano Jessica Bagger Assâad El Akremi Julie Camerman Gary J. Greguras Antonio Mladinic Carolina Moliner Kidok Nam Kjell Törnblom Kees Van den Bos 《决策行为杂志》2015,28(2):167-175
The present study examines the congruence of individuals' minimum preferred amounts of voice with the prospect theory value function across nine countries. Accounting for previously ignored minimum preferred amounts of voice and actual voice amounts integral to testing the steepness of gain and loss functions explicated in prospect theory, we use curve fitting to show that ratings of procedural justice fit prospect theory's value function specifically. Further, we investigate the form of this function across nine countries that range in power distance. Results suggest that the form of the value function is congruent with prospect theory, showing an S‐shaped curve that is steeper in the loss than in the gain domain. Further, this pattern is similar across countries. Theoretical and practical implications of these results for both decision making and organizational justice are discussed. Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
838.
This study explored the interrelated job search, unemployment, and coping literature (e.g., Leana & Feldman, 1990 ; McKee‐Ryan, Song, Wanberg, & Kinicki, 2005 ), in which the effects of age, length of unemployment, and coping behaviors on reemployment expectations were investigated. The study included unique demographics in which 65% were long‐term unemployed. Length of unemployment, networking comfort, and job search confidence were significant in a regression, and age was not. Regardless of age, reemployment expectations can be higher if the job seeker has higher levels of networking comfort and/or job search confidence or lower if they have been unemployed for a longer duration. 相似文献
839.
840.