全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1818篇 |
免费 | 16篇 |
专业分类
1834篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 58篇 |
2019年 | 50篇 |
2018年 | 25篇 |
2017年 | 27篇 |
2016年 | 49篇 |
2015年 | 52篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 152篇 |
2012年 | 67篇 |
2011年 | 55篇 |
2010年 | 39篇 |
2009年 | 39篇 |
2008年 | 58篇 |
2007年 | 74篇 |
2006年 | 56篇 |
2005年 | 60篇 |
2004年 | 71篇 |
2003年 | 65篇 |
2002年 | 53篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 19篇 |
1999年 | 24篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 26篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 25篇 |
1993年 | 27篇 |
1992年 | 22篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 33篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 20篇 |
1984年 | 27篇 |
1983年 | 21篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 27篇 |
1980年 | 25篇 |
1979年 | 29篇 |
1978年 | 32篇 |
1977年 | 19篇 |
1976年 | 22篇 |
1975年 | 22篇 |
1974年 | 27篇 |
1973年 | 17篇 |
1972年 | 17篇 |
1971年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有1834条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Sexual identity, attractions, and behavior among young sexual-minority women over a 2-year period 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Diamond LM 《Developmental psychology》2000,36(2):241-250
Previous research suggests that the sexual identities, attractions, and behaviors of sexual-minority (i.e., nonheterosexual) women change over time, yet there have been few longitudinal studies addressing this question, and no longitudinal studies of sexual-minority youths. The results of 2-year follow-up interviews with 80 lesbian, bisexual, and "unlabeled" women who were first interviewed at 16-23 years of age are reported. Half of the participants changed sexual-minority identities more than once, and one third changed identities since the first interview. Changes in sexual attractions were generally small but were larger among bisexuals and unlabeled women. Most women pursued sexual behavior consistent with their attractions, but one fourth of lesbians had sexual contact with men between the two interviews. These findings suggest that there is more fluidity in women's sexual identities and behaviors than in their attractions. This fluidity may stem from the prevalence of nonexclusive attractions among sexual-minority women. 相似文献
62.
63.
This study compared the strength of competing causal models in explaining the relationship between perceived support, enacted support, and social anxiety in adolescents. The social causation hypothesis postulates that social support causes social anxiety, whereas the social selection hypothesis postulates that social anxiety causes social support. The reciprocal model combines the two hypotheses by arguing that the causal relationship between social support and social anxiety is largely reciprocal. This study tests a modification of the reciprocal model by specifying perceptions of support as a mediating construct. Two waves of data with an interval of three months were collected on 357 college students. Structural equation modeling was used to compare the reciprocal and mediated reciprocal causal models. The study found some support for the mediated reciprocal model, but the magnitude of the relationships were weaker than expected. Limitations and suggestion for future research are discussed. 相似文献
64.
Leonardson GR Kemper E Ness FK Koplin BA Daniels MC Leonardson GA 《Psychological reports》2005,97(1):161-166
According to the Indian Health Service, substance abuse and Type 2 diabetes are serious problems among Native Americans. To assess substance use in a medical setting, valid screening tests are needed so the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT), a simple brief screen for excessive drinking, and the CAGE-adapted to Include Drugs (CAGE-AID) for identifying primary care patients with alcohol and drug disorders were given 50 Northern Plains American Indians with diabetes. Both are short, easy to administer, have good sensitivity and specificity, and can be easily incorporated into a medical history protocol or intake procedure. Reliability coefficients were above .90 and appeared to have sufficient concurrent and divergent validity indicated by moderate correlations with the General Well-being Schedule (rs=-.39 and -.36), the Family-Adaptation, Partnership, Growth, Affection, & Resolve (r =-.47 and -.36), and the Beck Depression Inventory-IT (r = .36 and .29). 相似文献
65.
Basic and applied research related to the effects of interspersing trials of maintenance (i.e., review) tasks among trials
of acquisition tasks on academic skill development is reviewed. In general, empirical research suggests that interspersing
procedures are effective in facilitating acquisition, learning rate, and maintenance. However, some inconsistency exists among
the data that suggests student learning, in some instances, may be impeded. Research also suggests that interspersing procedures
are not conducive to facilitating generalization. The discrepancy between various research findings and the consistent failure
of research on interspersing procedures to increase generalization is critically reviewed in relation to a hierarchical learning
theory set forth by Haring and Eaton (1978). From this hierarchical learning theory perspective, inconsistent results may
be better understood and predicted. Implications for current practice and directions for future research are also discussed. 相似文献
66.
The hindsight bias (e.g., Fischhoff, 1975) illustrates that outcome information can make people believe that they would have (or did) predict an outcome that they would not (or did not) actually predict. In two experiments, participants (N = 226) made a prediction immediately before receiving outcome information. Therefore, participants could not distort or misremember their predictions to make them align with the outcome information. In both experiments, participants distorted their reports of how certain they recalled having been in their prediction, how good of a basis they had for making the prediction, how long they took to make the prediction, and so forth. Experiment 2 showed that these effects were diminished when participants engaged in private thought about the upcoming questions prior to receiving outcome information, suggesting that the effect is not due to impression management concerns. 相似文献
67.
This study tested alternative factor models of the General Health Questionnaire-12 (GHQ-12), based on previous research findings, with a large sample using confirmatory factor analysis. An alternative models framework was used to test 6 factor analytic models. A 3-factor model was the best explanation of the sample data. The 3 factors were labeled Anxiety-Depression, Social Dysfunction, and Loss of Confidence. The model was found to be factorially invariant between men and women. The utility of the 3 subscales, as opposed to the total GHQ-12 score, is questioned as they appear to provide little information beyond that of a general factor. 相似文献
68.
Genetic Cancer Risk Assessment and Counseling: Recommendations of the National Society of Genetic Counselors 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Trepanier A Ahrens M McKinnon W Peters J Stopfer J Grumet SC Manley S Culver JO Acton R Larsen-Haidle J Correia LA Bennett R Pettersen B Ferlita TD Costalas JW Hunt K Donlon S Skrzynia C Farrell C Callif-Daley F Vockley CW;National Society of Genetic Counselors 《Journal of genetic counseling》2004,13(2):83-114
These cancer genetic counseling recommendations describe the medical, psychosocial, and ethical ramifications of identifying at-risk individuals through cancer risk assessment with or without genetic testing. They were developed by members of the Practice Issues Subcommittee of the National Society of Genetic Counselors Cancer Genetic Counseling Special Interest Group. The information contained in this document is derived from extensive review of the current literature on cancer genetic risk assessment and counseling as well as the personal expertise of genetic counselors specializing in cancer genetics. The recommendations are intended to provide information about the process of genetic counseling and risk assessment for hereditary cancer disorders rather than specific information about individual syndromes. Key components include the intake (medical and family histories), psychosocial assessment (assessment of risk perception), cancer risk assessment (determination and communication of risk), molecular testing for hereditary cancer syndromes (regulations, informed consent, and counseling process), and follow-up considerations. These recommendations should not be construed as dictating an exclusive course of management, nor does use of such recommendations guarantee a particular outcome. These recommendations do not displace a health care provider's professional judgment based on the clinical circumstances of a client. 相似文献
69.
This study tested a conceptual model of active coping among a predominantly middle-class sample (N = 92) of Black lesbians (ages 18 to 68) attending a Black lesbian retreat. F. B. Tyler's (1978) active coping dimension of psychosocial competence and L. P. Anderson, C. L. Eaddy, and E. A. Williams's (1990) psychosocial competence model for Black Americans emphasizing the role of individual and environmental factors provided the theoretical framework for the study. A theoretical model was developed to examine whether internal (self-esteem, race and lesbian identification) and external (social support, perceived available lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgendered resources) factors predicted active coping. Results confirmed that the model of internal and external factors was a statistically significant predictor of active coping. 相似文献
70.
Anderson MJ Barnes GW Briggs JF Ashton KM Moody EW Joynes RL Riccio DC 《Psychological reports》2004,94(2):437-443
The current experiment investigated ontogenetic forgetting on a novel object-recognition task similar to that of Besheer and Bevins. 18-day-old pups (n = 49) and adult (n = 29) rats were tested at two retention intervals (1 min. or 120 min.). By employing exclusion criteria which demanded minimum amounts of object exploration at training and test, the performance of 18-day-old pups but not that of adults was significantly impaired at 120 min. relative to 1 min. Analysis indicated that the ontogeny of the learning and memory measured in novel object recognition follows a developmental trend similar to that of other forms of learning, with older animals remembering more and thus performing better than younger animals. Unfortunately, given the extreme variability inherent to the task and large N necessary to achieve significance, the use of this task in studies of learning, memory, and development is discouraged. 相似文献