首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2054篇
  免费   72篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2020年   27篇
  2019年   26篇
  2018年   35篇
  2017年   38篇
  2016年   38篇
  2015年   35篇
  2014年   35篇
  2013年   192篇
  2012年   83篇
  2011年   71篇
  2010年   46篇
  2009年   47篇
  2008年   65篇
  2007年   84篇
  2006年   61篇
  2005年   70篇
  2004年   80篇
  2003年   67篇
  2002年   68篇
  2001年   35篇
  2000年   36篇
  1999年   33篇
  1998年   27篇
  1997年   29篇
  1996年   32篇
  1995年   27篇
  1994年   27篇
  1993年   31篇
  1992年   25篇
  1991年   23篇
  1990年   37篇
  1989年   19篇
  1988年   21篇
  1987年   23篇
  1986年   26篇
  1985年   36篇
  1984年   42篇
  1983年   27篇
  1982年   34篇
  1981年   43篇
  1980年   32篇
  1979年   30篇
  1978年   42篇
  1977年   20篇
  1976年   23篇
  1975年   31篇
  1974年   33篇
  1973年   20篇
  1972年   16篇
  1971年   16篇
排序方式: 共有2127条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
971.
The purpose of the current study was to investigate the effect of three procedures designed to promote the generalization of fluent responding of 12 addition facts to 12 related subtraction facts. The first procedure was completed to determine whether increases in the number of digits correct per minute computed on addition facts would generalize to related subtraction facts. If students were fluent in the targeted addition problems and no generalization was detected, a second procedure was implemented where students completed a conceptual lesson. If no generalization was observed during this phase, a third procedure was implemented where students completed addition problems presented in a cloze format. Results failed to demonstrate generalized fluency gains across any of the procedures. Discussion focuses on future directions of generalization research to further our understanding of the conditions needed for generalization to occur.  相似文献   
972.
The multiple memory systems hypothesis proposes that different types of learning strategies are mediated by distinct neural systems in the brain. Male and female mice were tested on a water plus-maze task that could be solved by either a place or response strategy. One group of mice was pre-exposed to the same context as training and testing (PTC) and the other group was pre-exposed to a different context (PDC). Our results show that the PTC condition biased mice to place strategy use in males, but this bias was dependent on the presence of ovarian hormones in females.The participation of different brain areas in place and response learning strategies has been studied extensively (White and McDonald 2002; Gold 2004; Mizumori et al. 2004). Place strategy is an allocentric navigation strategy that depends on extramaze cues. Response strategy is an egocentric navigation strategy based on proprioceptive cues. Inactivation of the hippocampus biased animals to response strategy use, and inactivation of the striatum biased animals to place strategy use (Packard and McGaugh 1996; Lee et al. 2008). Furthermore, glutamate infusion into the hippocampus strengthened place strategy use and, conversely, glutamate infusion into the striatum enhanced response strategy use (Packard 1999). These studies suggest that the hippocampus system mediates place strategy, while the striatum system mediates response strategy.Various factors can modulate learning strategy use, including training intensity (Packard and McGaugh 1996; Martel et al. 2007). A recent study investigated the influence of training on strategy use on a probe trial conducted 1 h after training (Martel et al. 2007). Male mice displayed enhanced place strategy use when trained on 12 or 22 trials compared with four trials, suggesting an effect of training intensity on strategy choice (Martel et al. 2007). This study further investigated the effect of pre-exposure to the training and testing context (PTC). Pre-exposure enhanced place strategy use in male mice after only four trials relative to animals pre-exposed to a different context (PDC). These results suggest that a sufficient exposure to the training and testing context promotes place strategy use in mice.The type of strategy used by rats is affected by both biological sex and gonadal steroids. Male rats typically employ a place strategy, especially during the early phase of training, on both land and water T-mazes (Packard and McGaugh 1992, 1996; Packard and Teather 1997). However, strategy use by female rats depends on hormonal conditions (Dohanich 2002; Dohanich et al. 2009). Place strategy is preferred by intact female rats on the day of proestrus when estradiol levels are elevated, and by ovariectomized rats treated with estradiol (Korol and Kolo 2002; Korol 2004; Korol et al. 2004). In contrast, response strategy is more often displayed by intact females on diestrus, and by ovariectomized females that did not receive estradiol replacement (Korol and Kolo 2002; Korol et al. 2004). To date, the effects of biological sex and gonadal steroids on learning strategy have not been studied in mice.In this study, we developed a modified version of the dual-solution water plus-maze task to further investigate the role of PTC compared with PDC in male and female mice. We hypothesized that strategy choice in both sexes would be dependent on context pre-exposure, and ovarian hormones would influence strategy choice in females. Our results show that PTC significantly enhanced place strategy use in male mice. Although there was no significant difference between PTC and PDC female mice, ovariectomy significantly reduced place strategy use in the PTC females, suggesting that ovarian hormones play a significant role in strategy use in female mice.Sixteen male and 39 female 129/Sve strain mice were obtained at 2–3 mo of age from Charles River Laboratories (Boston, MA). Mice were housed in groups of four on a 12/12 light/dark cycle (lights on at 07:00 h) with free access to food and water. All protocols followed the guidelines from a protocol approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of Tulane University in accordance with National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.Mice were pre-exposed for 5 min to the dry plus-maze either in the context of the subsequent training and testing (PTC), or in a different context in a different room (PDC), 30 min prior to the first training trial. The maze consisted of four clear Plexiglas arms (40 cm in length, 10 cm in width, and 40 cm in height). During the pre-exposure, mice were able to visit three arms of the maze. The rooms had different visual cues surrounding the maze. No extramaze cues were placed directly at the end of any arm. After the pre-exposure, the animal was placed in its home cage. The maze was wiped clean with 70% ethanol between trials.After pre-exposures, the maze was filled to 1.5 cm above the Plexiglas escape platform (15 cm in height) with room-temperature water colored opaque with white nontoxic tempera paint. Mice were trained in the water plus-maze task (Fig. 1A). The training was ended when the animals made six correct choices or reached nine trials. The animals that made fewer than four correct choices during training were not included in the study. Trials were continued until the mouse reached the platform or a maximum of 1 min. Each trial was separated by an intertrial interval of 4 min. Throughout the training trials, one arm (north) was blocked off by a white Plexiglas shield, creating a T-shaped maze. Mice were placed in the start arm of the maze (south) and were allowed to swim to the escape platform, which was consistently located in one arm of the maze for each animal and alternated between animals (east or west). Entry of the entire animal into the maze arm that contained the escape platform was scored as a correct response during the training trials, and entry of the entire animal into the maze arm that did not contain the escape platform was scored as an incorrect response. Mice were allowed to remain on the escape platform for 15 sec before being returned to their cages. Mice that failed to find the escape platform within 60 sec were manually guided to the platform. The water was distributed across all arms of the maze and the maze walls were wiped down to reduce intramaze cues between training and probe trials. One hour after training, mice were tested on a probe trial (Fig. 1B) in order to determine their relative use of “place” and “response” strategy. On the probe trial, mice were placed into the start arm 180° opposite the start arm used during training (i.e., end of the north arm) and were allowed to make an entry into either the east or west maze arm. The white Plexiglas shield blocked the south arm during the probe trial. Mice were designated as using place or response strategy based on the probe trial. Place strategy was designated as entry of the entire animal into the arm with the platform, and response strategy was designated as entry of the entire animal into the opposite arm.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.The effects of pre-exposure to the training and testing context (PTC) or to a different context (PDC) on strategy selection of male mice. (A) Schematic diagram of the water plus-maze. Mice were released from the south arm during training trials and from the north arm during the probe test. (B) More male mice used place strategy than response strategy when pre-exposed to the same context prior to training and testing (PTC, n = 5) compared with male mice pre-exposed to a different context (PDC, n = 7, P < 0.05). (C) Latency curves show the actual latency to escape to the platform. Two-way ANOVA (non-repeated measures) revealed no significant difference across training trials in escape latencies between PDC and PTC mice (P > 0.5), although a significant effect of trial indicated that mice reduced their escape latencies across trials (P < 0.001). Values represent mean ± S.E.M.Sixteen male mice were randomly divided into two groups based on pre-exposure context, PTC or PDC. Four of the 16 males were not included in the study for failure to reach criterion (four correct out of nine trials) or failure to escape to the platform due to floating, which is a behavior commonly seen in this strain (Wolfer et al. 1997). On the probe trial PTC males used the place strategy significantly more often than PDC males (P < 0.05, χ2 = 5.182, Fig. 1B). Four of five PTC males used place strategy, whereas only one of seven PDC males used place strategy. Pre-exposure of animals to the same or different context prior to training did not affect the latency to escape the platform during training. Latency to find the platform during training trials revealed a significant effect of trial (F(8,89) = 3.830, P = 0.0007, non-repeated measures two-way ANOVA) but not pre-exposure condition (F(1,89) = 0.103, P = 0.75, non-repeated measures two-way ANOVA; Fig. 1C). Moreover, the average swim speed of PDC male mice (6 ± 1.6 cm/sec, n = 7) was not significantly different than the average swim speed of PTC male mice (6 ± 2.5 cm/sec, n = 5; P = 0.34, t = 0.9 [t-test]). Together, these data suggest that the pre-exposure condition did not influence learning during the training period, but PTC did enhance place strategy use in the probe trial in male mice.Female mice at 3 mo of age were randomly divided into two groups: mice that would receive ovariectomy (Ovx), and a sham surgery group (Sh). Mice were anesthetized with a ketamine (80 mg/kg) and xylazine (8 mg/kg) mixture. The first group of mice (n = 20) received ovariectomy using a dorsolateral approach. The other group (n = 19) of female mice received sham surgery, which consisted of ovary exposure only. Animals were injected with the pain reliever, buprenorphine (5 mg/kg), immediately after the surgery. One week after the surgery, vaginal smears were collected from all females, including Ovx as handled controls, at the same time each morning by lavage to track their estrus cycles (Marcondes et al. 2002). After two regular cycles, Sh animals were trained and tested on the day of proestrus (high estradiol).Ovariectomy has been reported to affect anxiety levels (Walf et al. 2006), and anxiety levels may alter performance on water maze tasks. To assay possible anxiety differences between Sh and Ovx, female mice were tested on open field and elevated plus-maze (EPM) 2 wk after the surgery in a room different from the rooms used in water maze tasks. A single mouse was placed in the center of a white, Plexiglas chamber measuring 43 cm in length × 43 cm in width × 18 cm in height. The animal explored the novel environment for 15 min, and movements were monitored by a camera interfaced with a tracking system (US HVS Image). The area was divided into 16 virtual squares (10.75 × 10.75 cm) by the program, and the middle four squares were defined as the center area. The Plexiglas chamber was wiped clean with 70% ethanol between trials. The EPM consisted of four arms (5 cm in width × 30 cm in length) arranged perpendicularly in a plus shape and elevated 38 cm above the floor. Two arms were enclosed by 15.5-cm dark Plexiglas walls and two arms were open. Each animal was placed in the center of the EPM facing a closed arm and allowed to move freely for 5 min. Behavior was monitored by a camera interfaced with the tracking system.Animals with high anxiety levels tend to spend less time in the open arms of the EPM and in the center of the open field. The percent time spent in the open arms of the EPM by Ovx mice (37.9% ± 7.5%, n = 14) was not significantly different than the percent of time spent in the open arms by Sh mice (27.8% ± 6.2%, n = 15; P = 0.30, t = 1.1). The percent time spent in the center of the open field by Ovx mice (35.1% ± 7.1%, n = 14) was not significantly different from Sh mice (29.0% ± 7.1%, n = 15; P = 0.55, t = 0.61). These results indicate that ovarian hormones did not have a significant effect on the anxiety levels of the female mice tested in this study.Two weeks after the anxiety tests, the Ovx and Sh groups were divided randomly into two groups based on the pre-exposure context: Ovx PTC, Ovx PDC, Sh PTC, Sh PDC. Sh females with regular estrus cycles were trained and tested on the day of proestrus. Five Ovx and seven Sh animals were not included in the study because of floating, failing to reach criterion (four correct out of nine trials), or exhibiting irregular estrus cycles. Five of eight Sh PTC and only one of six Sh PDC females used place strategy; however, this difference was not significant (P > 0.05, χ2 = 2.94, Fig. 2A). Therefore, the pre-exposure condition did not significantly affect strategy use in females at proestrus.Open in a separate windowFigure 2.The effects of ovarian hormone status and pre-exposure to the training and testing (PTC) or to a different context (PDC) on strategy selection of female mice. (A) When pre-exposed to the same context prior to training and testing (PTC), more gonadally intact female mice at proestrus (Sh, n = 8) used place strategy than response strategy compared with ovariectomized female mice (Ovx, n = 8, P < 0.05). When pre-exposed to a context different than the training and testing context (PDC), gonadally intact female mice at proestrus (Sh, n = 6) and ovariectomized mice (Ovx, n = 6) used response strategy rather than place strategy. (B) Latency curves show the actual latency to escape to the platform. Two-way ANOVA (non-repeated measures) revealed no significant differences across training trials in escape latencies between sham and ovariectomized PTC and PDC mice (P > 0.5), although a significant effect of trial indicated that mice reduced their escape latencies across trials (P < 0.0001). Values represent mean ± S.E.M.Interestingly, ovariectomy did significantly affect strategy use in PTC females. Five of eight Sh PTC and only one of eight Ovx PTC females used place strategy (P < 0.05, χ2 = 4.267, Fig. 2A). One of six Sh PDC females and zero of the six Ovx PDC animals used place strategy (Fig. 2A). Therefore, both Sh and Ovx PDC females used response strategy, and ovarian hormones did not enhance place strategy use in PDC females (P > 0.05, χ2 = 1.09, Fig. 2A). Ovarian hormones did enhance place strategy use in PTC females. Furthermore, PTC did not enhance place strategy use in Ovx animals. Similar to males, there was a significant effect of training trial on latency to find the platform in female animals (F(8,189) = 10.32, P < 0.0001, Fig. 2B). Ovarian hormones or pre-exposure to either context also did not affect escape latency during training in PTC or PDC females (F(3,189) = 0.33, P = 0.80, Fig. 2B). In addition, there was no significant difference in the average swim speed between groups (F(3,14) = 0.15, P = 0.93, one-way ANOVA). The average swim speed for each group was as follows: Ovx PTC (5 ± 1.5 cm/sec, n = 5), Ovx PDC (5 ± 1.0 cm/sec, n = 4), Sh PTC (5 ± 1.8 cm/sec, n = 5), Sh PDC (6 ± 1.5 cm/sec, n = 4). The numbers of animals are lower because in some cases, speed was not measured. Together, these data suggest that ovarian hormones and pre-exposure condition did not influence learning during the training period, but ovarian hormones did enhance place strategy use in the probe trial in only PTC mice.Consistent with previous literature (Martel et al. 2007), we found that ∼80% of PTC males favored the use of place strategy. In addition, 63% of PTC females on proestrus also used place strategy. Ovx female mice used response strategy regardless of the pre-exposure condition. These results confirm that pre-exposure to the training and testing context significantly increased the use of place strategy or reduced response strategy in male mice, while female mice on proestrus were not significantly different than chance. Ovariectomy diminished the use of place strategy and enhanced response strategy use in our study, implicating ovarian hormones in strategy choice.Male rats rely initially on a hippocampus-dependent place strategy, and then switch to a striatum-based response strategy over training (Packard and McGaugh 1996; Packard 1999). This suggests that response strategy is incrementally learned with repeated exposure to the same task. However, a sufficient amount of time to explore the extramaze cues during or before training increased place strategy use in male mice (Martel et al. 2007). In addition, it has been proposed that the presence of an increased number of salient extramaze cues favors place strategy use in rats (Restle 1957). Therefore, it is possible that pre-exposing mice to the learning environment allowed them to build a cognitive map that facilitated the use of a spatial place strategy. Another possible advantage of pre-exposure for place strategy use is that it may reduce the impact of non-mnemonic factors, such as anxiety, on performance (Cain 1998). Indeed, it was shown that peripheral injection and infusion of anxiogenic drugs into the basolateral amygdala biased rats toward the use of response strategy (Packard 1999; Wingard and Packard 2008; Packard and Gabriele 2009).While PTC female mice were not significantly different than PDC female mice, ovariectomy did reduce place strategy choice in the PTC mice. An emerging theory proposes that estradiol modulates cognitive performance via shifts in learning strategy (Korol and Kolo 2002; Daniel and Lee 2004; Korol 2004; McElroy and Korol 2005; Zurkovsky et al. 2007). Shifts in strategy use occurred across the estrus cycle in rats such that the hippocampus-dependent strategy was favored when estradiol levels were high (Korol et al. 2004). Similarly, estradiol treatment in ovariectomized rats increased hippocampus-dependent place strategy and impaired response strategy use compared with nontreated ovariectomized females (Korol and Kolo 2002). Our results showing that the lack of ovarian hormones reduced place strategy and increased response strategy use in PTC mice are consistent with these studies.In summary, we present a new design to a traditional dual-solution land plus-maze. One issue with the land maze version of the task is that it requires food deprivation. The possible increase in the appetite as a result of ovariectomy (Wade 1975) or disruption in the estrus cycle in response to food deprivation (Daniel et al. 1999) could confound the results in females in tasks that present food reward. In order to avoid these confounds, we used a modified version of a water-escape plus-maze (Packard and Wingard 2004). In this design, compared with the water-escape plus-maze, the clear Plexiglas maze itself is filled with water, instead of placing the plus-maze into a water maze, allowing a better view of extramaze visual cues. However, unlike rats, mice tend to be prey animals when in the water; therefore they are highly motivated to escape the water (Francis et al. 1995; Van Dam et al. 2006). Consequently, the stressful nature of the task prevents mice from utilizing the spatial cues as efficiently (Frick et al. 2000). Therefore, we pre-exposed the mice to the maze while it was dry, allowing them to build a cognitive map before they were released in water. The water plus-maze is important not only for the design of future studies, but also for the evaluation of previous studies that investigated learning strategies using tasks dependent on food deprivation.  相似文献   
973.
This study explored the interrelated job search, unemployment, and coping literature (e.g., Leana & Feldman, 1990 ; McKee‐Ryan, Song, Wanberg, & Kinicki, 2005 ), in which the effects of age, length of unemployment, and coping behaviors on reemployment expectations were investigated. The study included unique demographics in which 65% were long‐term unemployed. Length of unemployment, networking comfort, and job search confidence were significant in a regression, and age was not. Regardless of age, reemployment expectations can be higher if the job seeker has higher levels of networking comfort and/or job search confidence or lower if they have been unemployed for a longer duration.  相似文献   
974.
Gary M. Simpson 《Dialog》2015,54(2):135-150
This article explores the intersection of missional ecclesiology and civil society studies and proposes that the Holy Spirit is calling emerging missional congregations to the vocation of public companions with God in global civil society. This congregational vocation of public companion entails four dimensions of missional congregations’ spiritual presence within civil society: perichoretic communion, prophetic speech, sapiential wisdom, and pacific action.  相似文献   
975.
The present study examines the congruence of individuals' minimum preferred amounts of voice with the prospect theory value function across nine countries. Accounting for previously ignored minimum preferred amounts of voice and actual voice amounts integral to testing the steepness of gain and loss functions explicated in prospect theory, we use curve fitting to show that ratings of procedural justice fit prospect theory's value function specifically. Further, we investigate the form of this function across nine countries that range in power distance. Results suggest that the form of the value function is congruent with prospect theory, showing an S‐shaped curve that is steeper in the loss than in the gain domain. Further, this pattern is similar across countries. Theoretical and practical implications of these results for both decision making and organizational justice are discussed. Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
976.
977.
Fundamental problems of environmental sustainability, including climate change and fisheries management, require collective action on a scale that transcends the political and cultural boundaries of the nation-state. Rational, self-interested neoclassical economic theories of human behavior predict tragedy in the absence of third party enforcement of agreements and practical difficulties that prevent privatization. Evolutionary biology offers a theory of cooperation, but more often than not in a context of discrimination against other groups. That is, in-group boundaries are necessarily defined by those excluded as members of out-groups. However, in some settings human’s exhibit behavior that is inconsistent with both rational economic and group driven cooperation of evolutionary biological theory. This paper reports the results of a non-cooperative game-theoretic exercise that models a tragedy of the commons problem in which groups of players may advance their own positions only at the expense of other groups. Students enrolled from multiple universities and assigned to different multi-university identity groups participated in experiments that repeatedly resulted in cooperative outcomes despite intergroup conflicts and expressions of group identity. We offer three possible explanations: (1) students were cooperative because they were in an academic setting; (2) students may have viewed their instructors as the out-group; or (3) the emergence of a small number of influential, ethical leaders is sufficient to ensure cooperation amongst the larger groups. From our data and analysis, we draw out lessons that may help to inform approaches for institutional design and policy negotiations, particularly in climate change management.  相似文献   
978.
Researchers in virtually every discipline rely on sophisticated proprietary software for their work. However, some researchers are unable to afford the licenses and instead procure the software illegally. We discuss the prohibition of software piracy by intellectual property laws, and argue that the moral basis for the copyright law offers the possibility of cases where software piracy may be morally justified. The ethics codes that scientific institutions abide by are informed by a rule-consequentialist logic: by preserving personal rights to authored works, people able to do so will be incentivized to create. By showing that the law has this rule-consequentialist grounding, we suggest that scientists who blindly adopt their institutional ethics codes will commit themselves to accepting that software piracy could be morally justified, in some cases. We hope that this conclusion will spark debate over important tensions between ethics codes, copyright law, and the underlying moral basis for these regulations. We conclude by offering practical solutions (other than piracy) for researchers.  相似文献   
979.
The extended mind hypothesis (EMH) is the claim that the mind can and does extend beyond the human body. Adams and Aizawa (A&A) contend that arguments for EMH commit a ‘coupling constitution fallacy’. We deny that the master argument for EMH commits such a fallacy. But we think that there is an important question lurking behind A&A's allegation: under what conditions is cognition spread across a tightly coupled system? Building on some suggestions from Haugeland, we contend that the system must exhibit a distinctive sort of semantic activity, semantic activity that the system as a whole takes responsibility for.  相似文献   
980.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号