首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   195篇
  免费   3篇
  2023年   2篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   5篇
  2018年   5篇
  2017年   4篇
  2016年   10篇
  2015年   7篇
  2014年   3篇
  2013年   25篇
  2012年   6篇
  2011年   7篇
  2010年   2篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   6篇
  2007年   6篇
  2006年   3篇
  2005年   4篇
  2004年   3篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   5篇
  2001年   5篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   9篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   3篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   2篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   1篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   3篇
  1971年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   3篇
  1966年   1篇
  1943年   1篇
排序方式: 共有198条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
161.
Garrett  Brian 《Philosophia》2017,45(4):1735-1739
Philosophia - In this discussion I argue that, given the possibility of travel to the past, eternalists face a dilemma. They must choose between fatalism and the denial of an intuitive claim about...  相似文献   
162.
Parenting self-efficacy (PSE) describes a parent’s belief in their ability to perform the parenting role successfully. Higher levels of PSE have consistently been shown to be correlated with a wide range of parenting and child outcomes. Consequently, many parenting interventions aim to improve PSE. PSE measurement has typically been via self-report measures. However, the wide range of available measures has resulted in their limited use, inconsistent terminology and ambiguous theoretical grounding. The purpose of this systematic review was to examine the psychometric and administrative qualities of the available PSE measures and offer clarity to the terminology and the theory underpinning their use so that the future use of PSE measures can be appropriate. Eleven electronic databases were searched. Articles were included if they introduced a new measure or were psychometric evaluations of an available measure of PSE for parents of children (from infancy until 18 years of age). Thirty-four measures were identified and their psychometric and administrative qualities were examined. Overall, the quality of the available measures was varied. Whilst this review makes recommendations regarding PSE measures for parents of infants through to adolescents, some caution should be applied when choosing the most appropriate measure. The theoretical grounding of each measure was clarified so that appropriate measures can be chosen under the relevant circumstances. The implications of refinement of the available measures are discussed and further research into improving PSE measurement is identified.  相似文献   
163.
164.
Although more commonly done by children, hopping appears to be a rich source of neuromuscular and biomechanical information on adults. Given prior research on the independent effects of hopping frequency and added mass, this study assessed whether these would interact to affect vertical stiffness, contact duration, and lower extremity kinematics during unipedal hopping. Vertical force and two-dimensional kinematics were measured in 10 healthy males hopping at three frequencies: their preferred hopping frequency and frequencies 20% higher and 20% lower, in two conditions with added mass (body mass+10% and body mass+20%). Vertical stiffness was directly related to hopping frequency, while hip flexion, knee flexion, and ankle dorsiflexion were inversely related to hopping frequency. Additional mass significantly increased ankle dorsiflexion and contact duration but did not significantly affect hip flexion, knee flexion, or vertical stiffness. The differential response of vertical stiffness to hopping frequency and added mass was consistent with predictions based on a mass-spring model. The interactive effect of frequency and added mass on the kinematics of the lower extremity and contact period were consistent with earlier studies of the independent effects of hopping frequency and added mass.  相似文献   
165.
Studies of agreement commonly occur in psychiatric research. For example, researchers are often interested in the agreement among radiologists in their review of brain scans of elderly patients with dementia or in the agreement among multiple informant reports of psychopathology in children. In this paper, we consider the agreement between two raters when rating a dichotomous outcome (e.g., presence or absence of psychopathology). In particular, we consider logistic regression models that allow agreement to depend on both rater- and subject-level covariates. Logistic regression has been proposed as a simple method for identifying covariates that are predictive of agreement (Coughlin et al., 1992). However, this approach is problematic since it does not take account of agreement due to chance alone. As a result, a spurious association between the probability (or odds) of agreement and a covariate could arise due entirely to chance agreement. That is, if the prevalence of the dichotomous outcome varies among subgroups of the population, then covariates that identify the subgroups may appear to be predictive of agreement. In this paper we propose a modification to the standard logistic regression model in order to take proper account of chance agreement. An attractive feature of the proposed method is that it can be easily implemented using existing statistical software for logistic regression. The proposed method is motivated by data from the Connecticut Child Study (Zahner et al., 1992) on the agreement among parent and teacher reports of psychopathology in children. In this study, parents and teachers provide dichotomous assessments of a child's psychopathology and it is of interest to examine whether agreement among the parent and teacher reports is related to the age and gender of the child and to the time elapsed between parent and teacher assessments of the child.The authors thank the Associate Editor and the referees for their helpful comments and suggestions. We also thank Gwen Zahner for use of data from the Connecticut Child Study, which was conducted under contract to the Connecticut Department of Children and Youth Services. This research was supported by grants HL 69800, AHRQ 10871, HL52329, HL61769, GM 29745, MH 54693 and MH 17119 from the National Institutes of Health.  相似文献   
166.
Sixteen sifakas (11 Propithecus verreauxi coquereli, 2 Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi form majori, and 3 Propithecus tattersalli) were videotaped as they fed on leaves in an arboreal context. The hand used to feed and the hand used to maintain postural stability was coded. For each subject, the lateral bias of the hand used to feed was opposite the hand used in postural support. Seven sifakas displayed no bias for feeding or posture-related hand use, 7 sifakas displayed significant feeding-related reach preferences for pulling branches to the mouth (5 left- and 2 right-hand preferences), and 9 sifakas exhibited significant hand preferences for postural support (2 left-, 7 right-hand preferent). Although these data do not strongly support the postural origins theory of behavioral lateralization, the modal preference pattern for sifakas that displayed significant hand preferences for posture and feeding involve a left bias for feeding and a right bias for postural support. s reserved).  相似文献   
167.
The power which narratives of mystical experience exert on our thinking derives from the central place of emotions within them. These narratives are crucial to the phenomenon of healing: not as cure, but as a means of accepting, understanding, and minimising suffering. Contemporary religious thought draws heavily on social theory but this paper shows not only that mystical emotions have escaped the attention of sociology, but also that sociology is unable to provide an adequate account of them or of their place in overcoming pain of all kinds. It does so by comparing three very different books about the emotions: Deborah Lupton's The Emotional Self (1998), Jack Barbalet's Emotions, Social Theory and Social Structure (1998), and the fourteenth century Revelations of Divine Love by Julian of Norwich. The last of these illuminates what the other two call the extra-discursive, embodied and unconscious dimensions of emotions. The first part of the paper makes a general comparison and the second turns to a range of specific emotions: shame and guilt, sorrow and depression, anger, fear, and anxiety, and the positive emotions of joy, love, and desire. This comparison helps explain how people live with chronic suffering and transcend it. It also shows that sociology, by making society itself a thing and elevating it to God-like status, cannot provide the critique of theology which was one of its original intentions, nor fully account for the healing influence of mystical phenomena.  相似文献   
168.
Across 6 studies, relationship closeness strongly influenced moral attribution. First, we found that moral attributions were stronger when one interacted with an intimate compared to less intimate other. Second, we examined attributions about those who treat some people well and others badly. Moral character attributions were worse when an actor harmed an intimate relationship partner compared to a less close relationship partner. Further, the effect of relationship partner on moral attributions was influenced by perceivers’ beliefs about one’s true character. Perceivers were apt to believe that one’s true character is revealed when interacting with close others.  相似文献   
169.
The generate–recognize model and the relational–item-specific distinction are two approaches to explaining recall. In this study, we consider the two approaches in concert. Following Jacoby and Hollingshead (Journal of Memory and Language 29:433–454, 1990), we implemented a production task and a recognition task following production (1) to evaluate whether generation and recognition components were evident in cued recall and (2) to gauge the effects of relational and item-specific processing on these components. An encoding task designed to augment item-specific processing (anagram-transposition) produced a benefit on the recognition component (Experiments 13) but no significant benefit on the generation component (Experiments 13), in the context of a significant benefit to cued recall. By contrast, an encoding task designed to augment relational processing (category-sorting) did produce a benefit on the generation component (Experiment 3). These results converge on the idea that in recall, item-specific processing impacts a recognition component, whereas relational processing impacts a generation component.  相似文献   
170.
Christian theology amidst post-communist societies finds itself in a precarious situation as it seeks to emerge from the competing social imaginaries of its totalitarian Soviet past and the democratic capitalism of its future. To do so, eschatological hope will need to spring eternal as it seeks understanding by faith in love of the triune God and its diverse neighbours while reckoning with its diasporic status. As such, this programmatic article succinctly circumscribes the meaning (hope), message (faith), and mission (love) of a diasporic Christian theology with an ecumenical vision predominately for university theological education under post-communist conditions. It seeks to give reason for the eschatological hope within (meaning) that is fixated on the resurrected Christ in the Spirit (message) for the wisdom and flourishing of humanity (mission).  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号