全文获取类型
收费全文 | 547篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
558篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 14篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 14篇 |
2013年 | 53篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 12篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 18篇 |
2007年 | 18篇 |
2006年 | 16篇 |
2005年 | 11篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 19篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 7篇 |
1969年 | 6篇 |
1968年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1950年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有558条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Sequences of nine binary auditory signals (dots and dashes) were presented to 20 subjects in Experiment I. The subjects were instructed to internally organize the signals into two-dimensional arrays. Visual patterns (letters) could be recognized in these imaginary arrays in both upright and rotated orientations. In Experiment II, a group of nine subjects, which was instructed to use spatial imagery of this kind, reproduced significantly longer sequences of signals than nine control subjects. By means of internal spatial organization, experimental subjects were able to reproduce sequences up to 45 signals in length, whereas control subjects recalled near the chance level for portions of sequences longer than nine signals. Three levels of information processing were postulated to account for the results, with spatial organization occupying a mediating level between acoustic and verbal levels. 相似文献
102.
E. James Kehoe Lloyd J. Frei Robert W. Tait I. Gormezano 《Behavior research methods》1975,7(2):183-186
Economical microprocessor-based computers have been recently developed by Motorola and Control Logic, Inc. Apparently, in response to the price competition, DEC has introduced the PDP-8/A, incorporating the memory characteristics of the microprocessor-based computers. Relative to older minicomputers, the microprocessor-based systems and the PDP-8/A “miniprocessor” require more time to execute an experimentally useful task. However, their real-time operating characteristics would appear to be sufficient for on-line use in behavioral control and data acquisition. The PDP-8/A’s compatibility with older models of the PDP-8 series enables it to use previously developed software, but, for purposes of software development, the microprocessor systems offer a more powerful architecture and instruction set. 相似文献
103.
An algorithm is described that computes relative frequencies of occurrence of all arbitrarily long substrings of sequential data, such as are obtained from experiments in learning/memory and verbal interaction. The algorithm offers high speed and provides systematization for the computation of empirical conditional probabilities. Use of this algorithm allows application of probabilistic and information theoretic disciplines to reveal dependencies between events separated arbitrarily in time. 相似文献
104.
The experiment (N= 312) tested the effects of two types of pretrial publicity (PTP) on the guilt verdicts of simulated jurors. Heinous PTP was manipulated by varying the degree to which the lurid details of a rape-murder were presented to prospective jurors. Prejudgement PTP varied in the extent to which it implied that the defendant was the perpetrator or the rape-murder. As predicted, PTP which was high in either heinousness or prejudgment increased the females' tendency to conclude-after the trial evidence-that the defendant was guilty of the crime. The biasing effect of prejudgment PTP, however, was significant only among females categorized as being of low IQ. In contrast, neither dimension of PTF' significantly influenced the guilt verdicts of male jurors. Several possible explanations of the sex's differential vulnerability to PTP were proposed. In addition, evidence was obtained that female jurors may have been derogating the rape-murder victim and defensively minimizing the gravity of her fate, as a result of motivations to believe in a just world. 相似文献
105.
An inexpensive circuit is described for EEG biofeedback or measurement. The circuit is designed to interface easily with most basic EEG machines. It taps the output from a single recording channel of the basic EEG instrument and provides the following for each EEG band (beta, alpha, theta, and delta): (a) very selective bandpass discrimination, (b) variable center frequency, (c) variable threshold at which feedback will be provided, or the measurement made, and (d) use with any biofeedback stimulus or measurement device that one wishes to switch contingent with S’s EEG rhythms. 相似文献
106.
Dan Lloyd 《Synthese》1987,70(1):23-78
Commonsense psychology and cognitive science both regularly assume the existence of representational states. I propose a naturalistic theory of representation sufficient to meet the pretheoretical constraints of a folk theory of representation, constraints including the capacities for accuracy and inaccuracy, selectivity of proper objects of representation, perspective, articulation, and efficacy or content-determined functionality. The proposed model states that a representing device is a device which changes state when information is received over multiple information channels originating at a single source. The changed state of a representing device is a representation. The unitary information source which would give rise to the information impinging on the representing device, and hence, give rise to the representation, is the content of the representation. The model meets the pretheoretic constraints, and also conforms to available neurobiological data for two invertebrate species. 相似文献
107.
108.
109.
Gareth J. Williams Clare Wood 《Advances in cognitive psychology / University of Finance and Management in Warsaw》2012,8(4):267-280
The role of suprasegmental information in reading processes is a growing area of
interest, and sensitivity to lexical stress has been shown to explain unique
variance in reading development. However, less is known about its role in
skilled reading. This study aimed to investigate the acoustic features of
suprasegmental information using a same/different cross-modal matching task.
Sixty-four adult participants completed standardized measures of reading
accuracy, reading speed, and comprehension and performed an experimental task.
The experimental task required the participants to identify whether non-speech
acoustic sequences matched the characteristics of written words. The findings
indicated differences in responses depending on where the lexical stress was
required for the word. Moreover, evidence was found to support the view that
amplitude information is part of the word knowledge retrieval process in skilled
reading. The findings are discussed relative to models of reading and the role
of lexical stress in lexical access. 相似文献
110.
The integration of a newly learned spoken word form with existing knowledge in the mental lexicon is characterized by the word form's ability to compete with similar-sounding entries during auditory word recognition. Here we show that although the mere acquisition of a spoken form is swift, its engagement in lexical competition requires an incubation-like period that is crucially associated with sleep. Words learned at 8 p.m. do not induce (inhibitory) competition effects immediately, but do so after a 12-hr interval including a night's sleep, and continue to induce such effects after 24 hr. In contrast, words learned at 8 a.m. do not show such effects immediately or after 12 hr of wakefulness, but show the effects only after 24 hr, after sleep has occurred. This time-course dissociation is best accommodated by connectionist and neural models of learning in which sleep provides an opportunity for hippocampal information to be fed into long-term neocortical memory. 相似文献