全文获取类型
收费全文 | 401篇 |
免费 | 78篇 |
国内免费 | 21篇 |
专业分类
500篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 14篇 |
2021年 | 12篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 25篇 |
2018年 | 22篇 |
2017年 | 20篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 11篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 27篇 |
2012年 | 19篇 |
2011年 | 29篇 |
2010年 | 22篇 |
2009年 | 25篇 |
2008年 | 15篇 |
2007年 | 40篇 |
2006年 | 37篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 22篇 |
2003年 | 10篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有500条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
451.
452.
453.
目的:了解高中生的绝望感、自杀意念状况,分析中学生自杀意念的保护性因素的作用机制,为自杀的心理干预提供必要的支持。方法:采用分层整群抽样法抽取在校高中生300人,利用应对效能量表、贝克绝望量表和自杀意念量表等进行调查分析。结果:自杀意念者的检出率为15%,数量分布无性别、年级和独生子女上的差异;应对效能、家庭亲密度和适应性和乐观主义与自杀意念呈显著负相关,对自杀意念有显著的负向影响;乐观主义在应对效能、家庭亲密度和适应性对自杀意念的影响种起部分中介作用。结论:应对效能、家庭亲密度和适应性和乐观主义是自杀意念的重要保护性因素;应对效能、家庭亲密度和适应性通过乐观主义对自杀意念产生影响。 相似文献
454.
Gao Dingguo Yang Zhiliang 《心理科学》2005,28(1):13-18
Recently,agreatdealofinteresthadbeenfocusedonim plicitmemory,whichischaracterisedastheeffectofapreviousl memorisedpieceofinformationonataskwithouttheexplicito deliberateattempttorecallthememory(UnderwoodandBright 1996).Itisbelievedthatpriming,amainindicatorofimplici memory,oftenoccurswhensubjectsdonotknowtherelation shipbetweentheprimeandtarget,andconsequently,ithasbee regardedanimplicitasopposedtoanexplicitformofmemor (Bowers,1996;GrafandSchacter,1985).Tulving(1983 andTulvingandThomson(… 相似文献
455.
To help academic associations in management develop, refine, and implement a code of ethics, we conducted a survey of management
educators’ perception of the ethicality of 142 specific behaviors in teaching, research, and service. The results of the survey
could be used to inform ethics committees of these associations regarding the level of acceptability of such conduct. The
potential value of our study for the Academy of Management or similar management associations lie in our (1) systematically
involving the members in building support for the code of ethics, (2) assessing members’ ethical judgments on both cross-sectional
and longitudinal bases so as to identify areas needing particular attention in ethical training, (3) providing an extensive
list of specific examples of questionable and potentially unethical behaviors so as to make it easier to implement the code,
and (4) providing a template survey document for potential use in involving more stakeholder groups in the development of
codes of ethics.
相似文献
M. Joseph SirgyEmail: |
456.
Psychologists have long been captivated by the perception of animacy – the fact that even simple moving shapes may appear to engage in animate, intentional, and goal-directed movements. Here we report several new types of studies of a particularly salient form of perceived animacy: chasing, in which one shape (the ‘wolf’) pursues another shape (‘the sheep’). We first demonstrate two new cues to perceived chasing – chasing subtlety (the degree to which the wolf deviates from perfectly ‘heat-seeking’ pursuit) and directionality (whether and how the shapes ‘face’ each other). We then use these cues to show how it is possible to assess the objective accuracy of such percepts, and to distinguish the immediate perception of chasing from those more subtle (but nevertheless real) types of ‘stalking’ that cannot be readily perceived. We also report several methodological advances. Previous studies of the perception of animacy have faced two major challenges: (a) it is difficult to measure perceived animacy with quantitative precision; and (b) task demands make it difficult to distinguish perception from higher-level inferences about animacy. We show how these challenges can be met, at least in our case study of perceived chasing, via tasks based on dynamic visual search (the Find-the-Chase task) and a new type of interactive display (the Don’t-Get-Caught! task). 相似文献
457.
This research investigated the development of affective decision-making (ADM) during early childhood, in particular role of difficulty in learning a gain/loss schedule. In Experiment 1, we administrated the Children's Gambling Task (CGT) to 60 Chinese children aged 3 and 4, replicating the results obtained by Kerr and Zelazo [Kerr, A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2004). Development of “hot” executive function: The Children's Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 55, 148–157]. In Experiment 2, the CGT was modified to make it easier for young children to learn the gain/loss schedule by increasing delayed loss frequency and magnitude in the disadvantageous deck, and a larger sample (181 children aged 3–5) was assessed. Age-differences between 3- and 4-year-olds, rather than 4- and 5-year-olds, showed that ADM develops rapidly between 3 and 4 years. The reduction of the difficulty in learning the gain/loss schedule provides the basis for an account of the development of young children's AMD. 相似文献
458.
时代精神与马克思主义法学 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
马克思主义的一个核心特性是历史的、实践的唯物主义,即客观真实地反映时代的精神,反映人类历史发展的本质.认识世界和改造世界的前提是实事求是,实事求是是马克思主义活的灵魂.在当今研究马克思主义的法学原理,特别是研究马克思主义法学的中国化道路,我认为必须克服教条主义,应该把时代精神对于马克思主义法学的新要求体现出来,不是照搬马克思和其他马克思主义经典理论家的本本,而是要把握他们关于法学理论的活的灵魂,师马克思主义之所以法. 相似文献
459.
It is generally acknowledged that people adopt different resolution strategies when facing conflicts with others. However, the mechanisms of conflict resolution are still unclear and under researched, in particular within the context of Chinese adolescents' same-sex friendship relations. Thus, the present study investigated the mediator role of conflict resolution strategies in the relationship between regulatory foci and friendship satisfaction for the first time. 653 Chinese adolescents completed the regulatory foci, conflict resolution style, and friendship satisfaction measures. The results of the structure equation modeling showed that while promotion focus was positively associated with problem-solving and compliance, prevention focus was positively associated with withdrawal and conflict engagement. In addition, problem-solving mediated the relationship between promotion focus and friendship satisfaction, and conflict engagement mediated the relationship between prevention focus and friendship satisfaction. These findings contribute to understanding Chinese adolescents' use of conflict resolution strategies as well as the relationship between regulatory foci and behavioral strategies in negative situations. 相似文献
460.
Yu Gao Yonglin Huang Xiaobo Li 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2017,39(1):1-14
Evidence has suggested that neurobiological deficits combine with psychosocial risk factors to impact on the development of antisocial behavior. The current study concentrated on the interplay of prenatal maternal stress and autonomic arousal in predicting antisocial behavior and psychopathic traits. Prenatal maternal stress was assessed by caregiver’s retrospective report, and resting heart rate and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) were measured in 295 8- to 10-year-old children. Child and caregiver also reported on child’s antisocial behavior and psychopathic traits. Higher prenatal maternal stress was associated with higher caregiver-reported antisocial and psychopathy scores, even after the concurrent measure of social adversity was controlled for. As expected, low heart rate and high RSA were associated with high antisocial and psychopathic traits. More importantly, significant interaction effects were found; prenatal stress was positively associated with multiple dimensions of psychopathic traits only on the conditions of low arousal (e.g., low heart rate or high RSA). Findings provide further support for a biosocial perspective of antisocial and psychopathic traits, and illustrate the importance of integrating biological with psychosocial measures to fully understand the etiology of behavioral problems. 相似文献