全文获取类型
收费全文 | 499篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
503篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 11篇 |
2019年 | 18篇 |
2018年 | 18篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 35篇 |
2015年 | 22篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 52篇 |
2012年 | 24篇 |
2011年 | 31篇 |
2010年 | 15篇 |
2009年 | 13篇 |
2008年 | 12篇 |
2007年 | 17篇 |
2006年 | 22篇 |
2005年 | 16篇 |
2004年 | 14篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1955年 | 1篇 |
1953年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有503条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
431.
Phenomena in a variety of verbal tasks—for example, masked priming, lexical decision, and word naming—are typically explained
in terms of similarity between word-forms. Despite the apparent commonalities between these sets of phenomena, the representations
and similarity measures used to account for them are not often related. To show how this gap might be bridged, we build on
the work of Hannagan, Dupoux, and Christophe, Cognitive Science 35:79-118, (2011) to explore several methods of representing visual word-forms using holographic reduced representations and to evaluate them
on their ability to account for a wide range of effects in masked form priming, as well as data from lexical decision and
word naming. A representation that assumes that word-internal letter groups are encoded relative to word-terminal letter groups
is found to predict qualitative patterns in masked priming, as well as lexical decision and naming latencies. We then show
how this representation can be integrated with the BEAGLE model of lexical semantics (Jones & Mewhort, Psychological Review 114:1–37, 2007) to enable the model to encompass a wider range of verbal tasks. 相似文献
432.
Theories of social surrogacy and embodied cognition assume that cognitive associations with nonhuman stimuli can be affectively charged. In the current research, we examined whether the "comfort" of comfort foods comes from affective associations with relationships. Two experiments support the hypotheses that comfort foods are associated with relationships and alleviate loneliness. Experiment 1 found that the consumption of comfort foods automatically activates relationship-related concepts. Experiment 2 found that comfort foods buffer against belongingness threats in people who already have positive associations with relationships (i.e., are secure in attachment style). Implications for social surrogacy, need to belong, embodied cognition, and eating behavior are discussed. 相似文献
433.
434.
Diaz GJ Fajen BR Phillips F 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2012,38(4):848-864
People can often anticipate the outcome of another person's actions based on visual information available in the movements of the other person's body. We investigated this problem by studying how goalkeepers anticipate the direction of a penalty kick in soccer. The specific aim was to determine whether the information used to anticipate kick direction is best characterized as local to a particular body segment or distributed across multiple segments. In Experiment 1, we recorded the movements of soccer players as they kicked balls into a net. Using a novel method for analyzing motion capture data, we identified sources of local and distributed information that were reliable indicators of kick direction. In Experiments 2 and 3, subjects were presented with animations of kickers' movements prior to foot-to-ball contact and instructed to judge kick direction. Judgments were consistent with the use of distributed information, with a possible small contribution of local information. 相似文献
435.
Veronika Brandsttter Naomi Ellemers Elena Gaviria Francesca Giosue Pascal Huguet Marceline Kroon Pascal Morchain Margot Pujal Monica Rubini Gabriel Mugny Juan A. Perez 《European journal of social psychology》1991,21(3):199-211
In a 2 × 2 design, 85 subjects were asked to estimate the size of angles (direct influence) that were either 90 or 85°, after being confronted with incorrect judgements of a majority (88 per cent) or a minority (12 per cent) of people estimating the angles at 50°. Additionally, pre- and post-test measures were used to establish indirect influence on subjects' judgements pertaining to acute angles (i.e. on the estimation of the length of lines constituting the angles, and on the imaginary weight of figures represented by these angles). Overall, little direct influence is observed. This may partly be due to the introduction of a denial of the credibility of the source in all conditions. In fact, some evidence of direct influence is only found in the majority–85° angles condition. An instance of indirect influence (on the estimation of length of lines) appeared as the result of a majority stance when the angles in the experimental phase were 90°. When these angles were 85°, indirect minority influence (on the estimation of weight of figures) was observed. These effects had been predicted on the basis of the hypothesis stating that indirect majority influence would be possible when subjects expected consensus on the correct response (in the 90° angles condition), without being able to reach consensus at the manifest level (because of the denial and the restriction imposed by the clear shape of 90° angles). Indirect minority influence was hypothesized to be stronger in a situation that allows for diverse responses (i.e. for 85° angles). 相似文献
436.
Hansen KG 《Journal of personality assessment》2007,89(Z1):S52-S56
This article provides a normative study documenting how 75 Danish nonpatient 9-year-old children respond to the Rorschach test. The children were selected randomly from nine different parts of Denmark and tested with the Rorschach by 10 different psychologists. All examiners were familiar with the Comprehensive System (CS; Exner, 2003), and before the data collection they participated in a 3-day workshop performed by the research group that focused on administration, inquiry, and scoring issues. Among the results are an average R of 23.6 and an average Lambda of 2.01 (median = 1.18). Slightly more than 60% of the sample had an Avoidant style (63%); 41% had a CDI of 4 or 5; and 33% were positive on the HVI. In terms of perception and thinking, average values were .44 for X+%, .27 for X-%, and 9.4 for the WSum6. 相似文献
437.
438.
Javier Virues-Ortega Neil Martin Gabriel Schnerch Jesús ángel Miguel García Fae Mellichamp 《The Behavior analyst / MABA》2015,38(1):127-135
As the field of behavior analysis expands internationally, the need for comprehensive and systematic glossaries of behavioral terms in the vernacular languages of professionals and clients becomes crucial. We created a Spanish-language glossary of behavior-analytic terms by developing and employing a systematic set of decision-making rules for the inclusion of terms. We then submitted the preliminary translation to a multi-national advisory committee to evaluate the transnational acceptability of the glossary. This method led to a translated corpus of over 1200 behavioral terms. The end products of this work included the following: (a) a Spanish-language glossary of behavior analytic terms that are publicly available over the Internet through the Behavior Analyst Certification Board and (b) a set of translation guidelines summarized here that may be useful for the development of glossaries of behavioral terms into other vernacular languages. 相似文献
439.
Yuan Cheng Gabriel Nudelman Jianhong Ma Kathleen Otto 《International journal of psychology》2024,59(1):74-85
This research examined whether employees' personal belief in a just world (BJW) is associated with their organisational loyalty and whether this relationship is statistically mediated by organisational trust. To test these hypotheses, we conducted two studies with employees from China (study 1, N = 314) and Germany (study 2, N = 189). The results from both studies supported the proposed model. In addition, study 2 revealed that the relationship between BJW and organisational loyalty persisted when controlling for global personality traits. These suggest that managers and organisations may increase employees' loyalty by providing an environment that fosters their sense of justice and trust. 相似文献
440.
Jessica M. Gerton Keiko Aoyagi Gabriel A. León James Bludworth Sean Spille Jenny Holzapfel 《Counselling and Psychotherapy Research》2023,23(1):188-197
The onset of the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic necessitated a rapid transition of mental health services from in-person to telehealth counselling. Despite the far-reaching impact of this transition, we know little about the impact of this transition on outcomes for clients working with counsellor trainees. The present study utilised longitudinal data collected from a counselling training centre at a major U.S. university to compare client ratings of depression, anxiety and working alliance across 1) in-person services delivered (i.e. pre-pandemic) and 2) telehealth services delivered after the pandemic began (i.e. intra-pandemic) among the same group of clients. Results support our hypothesis that changes in clients' self-reports would be generally equivalent across in-person and telehealth services. Depression and anxiety symptoms decreased, and working alliance tended to increase during both in-person and telehealth care; however, these trends were only statistically significant during telehealth services. Limitations related to sample size (N = 15 clients; up to 17 sessions per client) and low statistical power are discussed. Nonetheless, this study supports the growing body of literature supporting the efficacy of telehealth counselling services. We provide suggestions for future telehealth research and discuss implications for counsellor training. 相似文献