全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4253篇 |
免费 | 69篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
4329篇 |
出版年
2012年 | 419篇 |
2011年 | 493篇 |
2010年 | 128篇 |
2009年 | 42篇 |
2008年 | 405篇 |
2007年 | 383篇 |
2006年 | 354篇 |
2005年 | 303篇 |
2004年 | 266篇 |
2003年 | 228篇 |
2002年 | 235篇 |
2001年 | 163篇 |
2000年 | 219篇 |
1999年 | 79篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1965年 | 11篇 |
1964年 | 9篇 |
1963年 | 7篇 |
1959年 | 15篇 |
1958年 | 29篇 |
1957年 | 34篇 |
1956年 | 35篇 |
1955年 | 31篇 |
1954年 | 34篇 |
1953年 | 32篇 |
1952年 | 25篇 |
1951年 | 26篇 |
1950年 | 17篇 |
1949年 | 20篇 |
1948年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有4329条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
251.
We investigated the unimanual actions of a biological family group of twelve western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) using a methodological approach designed to assess behavior within social context from a bottom-up perspective. Measures
of both the lateralization of unimanual actions (left, right) and the target of the action (animate, inanimate) were assessed
during dual, synchronized video observations of naturalistic behavior. This paper demonstrates a corelationship between handedness
and the animate quality of the target object. Analyses demonstrated a significant interaction between lateralized unimanual
actions and target animacy and a right-hand bias for actions directed toward inanimate targets. We suggest that lateralized
motor preference reflects the different processing capabilities of the left and right hemispheres, as influenced by the emotive
(animate) and/or functional (inanimate) characteristics of the target, respectively. 相似文献
252.
It is widely known that gaze plays an essential role in communicative interactions. Domestic dogs tend to look at the human
face in situations of conflict and uncertainty. This study compares the gaze of shelter and pet dogs during acquisition and
extinction phases in a situation involving a reward in sight but out of reach. Even though no significant differences between
the groups were recorded during acquisition, gaze duration decreased in both groups during extinction, with shelter dogs showing
a significant shorter duration. This could be related to their different living conditions and to the fact that through their
ordinary everyday interactions, pet dogs have more opportunities to learn to persist in their communicative responses when
they do not get what they want. These results highlight the relevance of learning experiences during ontogeny, which would
therefore modulate communicative responses. 相似文献
253.
Bakken IJ Wenzel HG Götestam KG Johansson A Oren A 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2009,50(2):121-127
Most Norwegians are Internet users. We conducted a stratified probability sample study (Norway, 2007, age-group 16–74 years, N = 3,399, response rate 35.3%, 87.1% Internet users) to assess the prevalence of Internet addiction and at-risk Internet use by the Young Diagnostic Questionnaire (YDQ). The prevalence of Internet addiction (YDQ score 5–8) was 1.0% and an additional 5.2% were at-risk Internet users (YDQ score 3–4). Internet addiction and at-risk Internet use was strongly dependent on gender and age with highest prevalences among young males (16–29 years 4.1% and 19.0%, 30–39 years 3.3% and 10.7%). Logistic regression showed that male gender, young age, university level education, and an unsatisfactory financial situation were factors positively associated with "problematic Internet use" (at-risk and addicted use combined). Time spent on the Internet and prevalence of self-reported sleeping disorders, depression, and other psychological impairments increased linearly with YDQ score. Problematic Internet use clearly affects the lives of many people. 相似文献
254.
Marchetti G 《Cognitive processing》2009,10(1):7-40
The analysis of time is vitiated very often by circularity: several disciplines, such as psychology, linguistics, and neurosciences,
analyze time by using concepts or terms which already contain in themselves, or are based, on the experience and notion of
time (as when, for example, time is defined as “duration”, or when our ability to estimate durations is explained by resorting
to the notion of an internal clock). Some detailed examples of circularity in the analysis of time are given here and examined.
A way out of circularity is then given: it is represented by the proposal of attentional semantics (AS) of considering words
and their meanings in terms of the aim they serve, and the means and processes developed and implemented in order to achieve
that aim. According to AS, the main aim of words is that of indicating to, and eliciting in, the listener or reader a specific
conscious experience: namely, the conscious experience referred to by their meanings. Words achieve their main aim by conveying
the condensed instructions on the attentional operations one has to perform if one wants to consciously experience what is
expressed through and by them. By describing the conscious experiences elicited by words in terms of the attentional operations
that are responsible for the production of such conscious experiences, AS offers an a-linguistic counterpart to language,
and therefore an effective way out of circularity. Following in footsteps of Mach (Contributions to the analysis of the sensations,
1890), but slightly revising his hypothesis, AS defines time-sensation as the perception of the effort made, or alternatively
the nervous energy expended, by the organ of attention when performing a “temporal activity” (for instance, estimating duration),
that is, when one’s own attention is focused in a continuous and incremental way on the conscious product of the (“non-temporal”)
activity performed by means of another portion of one’s attention.
相似文献
Giorgio MarchettiEmail: Email: |
255.
21世纪第一个10年过去了。膜拜团体、宗教异端和他们“超凡能力”教主20多年前制造的光怪陆离的故事淡出了人们的视线.那些令人唏嘘的脸庞、痛苦扭曲的身体、悲痛惨绝的事件也渐渐被人遗忘。远离了烽火硝烟,穿越地域和时空,听听社会科学工作者研究和解读“膜拜”.丰富我们的认知和阅历.在纷纭复杂的环境中健康生活,善待自己和家人. 相似文献
256.
Peer observation, while often used in other professions, has not been formally applied in genetic counseling. The objective
of this study was to pilot a method of peer evaluation whereby genetic counselors observed, and were observed by, each other
during patient interaction. All of the available genetic counselors participated in both rounds of the pilot study (six in
round one, seven in round two). The genetic counselors that observed the session used an observation room. Most participants
reported learning a new skill. Sensitivity to, and comfort with, the feedback process improved. We conclude that Peer-Observed
Interaction and Structured Evaluation (POISE) provides an opportunity to refresh counseling approaches and develop feedback
skills without causing undue team discord. This new approach to peer supervision in genetic counselling offers a live observation
approach for genetic counsellor supervision. 相似文献
257.
The present study examines the influence of hierarchical level on category representation. Three computational models of representation – an exemplar model, a prototype model and an ideal representation model – were evaluated in their ability to account for the typicality gradient of categories at two hierarchical levels in the conceptual domain of clothes. The domain contains 20 subordinate categories (e.g., trousers, stockings and underwear) and an encompassing superordinate category (CLOTHES). The models were evaluated both in terms of their ability to fit the empirical data and their generalizability through marginal likelihood. The hierarchical level was found to clearly influence the type of representation: For concepts at the subordinate level, exemplar representations were supported. At the superordinate level, however, an ideal representation was overwhelmingly preferred over exemplar and prototype representations. This finding contributes to the increasingly dominant view that the human conceptual apparatus adopts both exemplar representations and more abstract representations, contradicting unitary approaches to categorization. 相似文献
258.
Campoy G 《Acta psychologica》2011,(1):135-142
Two experiments investigated the possibility that the word-length effect in short-term memory (STM) is a consequence of long words generating a greater level of retroactive interference than shorter words. In Experiment 1, six-word lists were auditorily presented under articulatory suppression for immediate serial reconstruction of only the first three words. These three words were always drawn from a single set of middle-length words, whereas the last three positions were occupied by either short or long interfering words. The results showed worse memory performance when the to-be-remembered words were followed by long words. In Experiment 2, a recent-probes task was used, in which recent negative probes matched a target word in trial n-2. The results showed lower levels of proactive interference when trial n-1 involved long words instead of short words, suggesting that long words displaced previous STM content to a greater extent. By two different experimental approaches, therefore, this study shows that long words produce more retroactive interference than short words, supporting an interference-based account for the word-length effect. 相似文献
259.
Steward WT Chandy S Singh G Panicker ST Osmand TA Heylen E Ekstrand ML 《Psychology, health & medicine》2011,16(1):74-85
Previous research has shown that HIV stigma in India can be characterized by a framework dividing manifestations into enacted (discrimination), vicarious (hearing stories of discrimination), felt normative (perceptions of stigma's prevalence), and internalized stigma (personal endorsement of stigma beliefs). We examined whether this framework could explain associations among stigma, efforts to avoid HIV serostatus disclosure, and depression symptoms in a cohort of 198 HIV-infected individuals from Southern India who were followed up for one year as part of a study of antiretroviral adherence. Prior studies had suggested that disclosure avoidance was a primary outcome of stigma and that impaired well-being was a primary outcome of disclosure avoidance. Analyses from our longitudinal research revealed that the pattern of associations among stigma, disclosure avoidance, and depression symptoms remained consistent over time. Enacted and vicarious stigmas were correlated with felt normative stigma beliefs. In turn, felt normative stigma was correlated with disclosure avoidance. And, enacted stigma, internalized stigma, and disclosure avoidance were all associated with depression symptoms. However, even though the overall framework held together, internalized stigma and depression symptoms dropped significantly over time while other components remained unchanged. These findings suggest that, although HIV stigma may limit disclosure, it does not invariably lead to psychological maladjustment. Amidst ongoing perceptions and experiences of stigma, HIV-positive individuals can achieve significant improvements in their acceptance of the disease and in mental well-being. 相似文献
260.