首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4405篇
  免费   105篇
  国内免费   22篇
  4532篇
  2020年   8篇
  2014年   8篇
  2012年   428篇
  2011年   498篇
  2010年   135篇
  2009年   58篇
  2008年   420篇
  2007年   411篇
  2006年   367篇
  2005年   307篇
  2004年   273篇
  2003年   234篇
  2002年   240篇
  2001年   174篇
  2000年   222篇
  1999年   82篇
  1998年   28篇
  1997年   18篇
  1996年   13篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   12篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   9篇
  1991年   11篇
  1990年   11篇
  1989年   11篇
  1988年   7篇
  1985年   9篇
  1984年   19篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   15篇
  1981年   13篇
  1980年   8篇
  1979年   11篇
  1978年   8篇
  1977年   7篇
  1965年   12篇
  1964年   9篇
  1959年   16篇
  1958年   30篇
  1957年   34篇
  1956年   35篇
  1955年   31篇
  1954年   34篇
  1953年   32篇
  1952年   25篇
  1951年   26篇
  1950年   17篇
  1949年   20篇
  1948年   11篇
排序方式: 共有4532条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
211.
Gender differences are evident in the etiology and maintenance of aggressive behavior (which is on the increase for both male and female youths), but the explanatory models that have been proposed, including social learning theory and information processing models, are more appropriate for boys than for girls. In an attempt to elucidate processes underlying aggression, particularly for girls, the authors explored locus of control as a cognitive variable possibly related to aggressive behavior. The relationships between teacher-reported aggression and 3 types of locus of control (internal, powerful other, and unknown) for success and failure experiences were investigated. For girls, aggressive behavior was positively related to internal and unknown locus of control. In contrast, boys' aggressive behavior was unrelated to internal locus of control beliefs and negatively related to external locus of control beliefs. Possible explanatory mechanisms for the relationship between locus of control beliefs and aggression are discussed. The present study contributes to the understanding of aggression in girls and points to different processes underlying girls' and boys' aggression.  相似文献   
212.
Data from two multi-ethnic prospective studies of African American, Latina, and non-Hispanic White pregnant women were used to examine the influence of contextual factors on social support processes during pregnancy. Multiple types of support (perceived support, received support, support satisfaction, network support) and sources of support (baby's father, family, friends) were assessed. The role of ethnicity in social support was examined after controlling for the contribution of related contextual factors (SES, marital status, age, parity, employment) to these processes. The impact of ethnicity and related contextual factors differed across sources of social support. Ethnic differences in support from family and friends, but not from the baby's father, emerged. However, marital status was a consistent predictor of support from the baby's father, and SES was a consistent predictor of support from friends. Overall, the findings of two studies suggest that although ethnicity is associated with support from friends and family, other contextual factors, such as marital status and SES, influence support processes during pregnancy.  相似文献   
213.
Kellas G  Vu H 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》1999,6(3):511-7; discussion 518-22
Using a self-paced reading task, Kellas, Martin, Yehling, Herman, and Vu (1995) demonstrated that strength of context can modulate the effects of meaning frequency. Binder and Rayner (1998) initially replicated the results, using eye-tracking methodology. On further examination of the stimuli, Binder and Rayner eliminated 43% of the stimulus set and found that context strength failed to modulate meaning frequency. Binder and Rayner's initial replication of Kellas et al. and the convergence of results between their two main experiments established the validity of self-paced reading as a measure of on-line reading, when compared with eye-tracking methodology. However, their central conclusion, that context strength cannot modulate the subordinate bias effect, is open to question. In this commentary, we examine the criteria adopted to exclude items from our homonym set and discuss the issue of local versus published norms. We also discuss the issue of context strength, as related to the specific rating procedures employed. Finally, we conclude that strong context can, in fact, eliminate the subordinate bias effect and that the context-sensitive model can more fully account for the available data on lexical ambiguity resolution.  相似文献   
214.
215.
In 1929 Viktor P. Protopopov began to replicate E. L. Thorndike's animal experiments on habit acquisition. To determine the conditions necessary for habit formation, Protopopov used the natural experiment method, in which dogs encountered environments that prevented them from reaching a stimulus-bait. Not all dogs acquired the behavior necessary for obtaining the bait. Explaining the results within the framework of the Pavlovian theory of higher nervous activity, Protopopov concluded that habits were acquired when an active animal provoked by a bait-stimulus encountered an environmental barrier. The dogs tried a series of phylogenetic behaviors until the stimulus-bait was reached. The latter movements were retained, forming an ontogenetic habit. The dogs also learned not to produce the unsuccessful movements. In accord with the Pavlovian theory, individual differences in habit formation were related to temperament types. A critique of the Thorndikian Law of Effect is provided in terms of the Pavlovian theory of higher nervous activity.  相似文献   
216.
217.
218.
Ⅱ胡戈·丁格雷尔(Hugo Dingler)生于1881年,卒于1954年。1901—1904年,他在慕尼黑大学学习数学和物理。1905年到1906年夏季,他在格丁根跟随D.希耳伯特和F.克莱因(Klein)学习数学,跟随胡塞尔学习哲学,这是他第一次学哲学。1907年他在墓尼黑在A.福斯(Voss)的指导下以《普通平面变形》论文取得了博士学位。1912年也只是在墓尼黑大学,获得了“方法、教导法和数学史”这一无  相似文献   
219.
This study tested and refined a framework that proposes a mechanism for retrieving alternative causes and disabling conditions (Cummins, 1995) during reasoning. Experiment 1 examined the relation between different factors affecting retrieval. The test revealed high correlations between the number of possible alternative causes or disabling conditions and their strength of association and plausibility. Experiment 2 explored the hypothesis that due to a more extended search process, conditional inferences would last longer when many alternative causes or disabling conditions were available. Affirmation of the consequent (AC) and modus ponens (MP) latencies showed the hypothesized pattern. Denial of the antecedent (DA) and modus tollens (MT) inferences did not show latency effects. The experiment also identified an effect of the number of disabling conditions on AC and DA acceptance. Experiment 3 measured efficiency of disabler retrieval by a limited time, disabler generation task. As predicted, better disabler retrieval was related to lower acceptance of the MP and MT inferences.  相似文献   
220.
十五岁月指弹间, 六十期刊未偷闲。捷报葡园结硕果, 导引群羊饮甘泉。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号