全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4373篇 |
免费 | 71篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
4450篇 |
出版年
2012年 | 441篇 |
2011年 | 506篇 |
2010年 | 134篇 |
2009年 | 46篇 |
2008年 | 406篇 |
2007年 | 386篇 |
2006年 | 356篇 |
2005年 | 305篇 |
2004年 | 273篇 |
2003年 | 228篇 |
2002年 | 237篇 |
2001年 | 166篇 |
2000年 | 228篇 |
1999年 | 95篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 49篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 17篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1965年 | 11篇 |
1964年 | 9篇 |
1963年 | 6篇 |
1959年 | 15篇 |
1958年 | 28篇 |
1957年 | 34篇 |
1956年 | 35篇 |
1955年 | 31篇 |
1954年 | 34篇 |
1953年 | 32篇 |
1952年 | 25篇 |
1951年 | 26篇 |
1950年 | 17篇 |
1949年 | 20篇 |
1948年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有4450条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
Moretti R Torre P Antonello RM Fabbro F Cazzato G Bava A 《Perceptual and motor skills》2003,97(1):215-229
Writing is a complex process requiring visual memory, attention, phonological and semantic operations, and motor performance. For that reason, it can easily be disturbed by interfering with attention, memory, by interfering subvocalization, and so on. With 16 female third-year students (23.4 +/- 0.8 yr.) from the University of Trieste, we investigated the production of errors in three experimental conditions (control, articulatory suppression, and tapping). In the articulatory suppression condition, the participants produced significantly more linguistic impairments (such as agrammatism, unrelated substitutions, sentence omissions, and semantically deviant sentences), which are similar to linguistic impairments found in aphasia. On the tapping condition there were more perseverations, deletions, and substitutions of both letters and words. These data suggest that writing is not an automatic skill. Only after many years of experience and practice of processing information (through cortical to subcortical channels) can writing be considered an automatic skill. Limited experimental conditions can disrupt the writing system of normal subjects, probably interfering with the cortical to subcortical loops, and link normality to pathology. 相似文献
182.
This study tested the effects of different hypertension models on active avoidance learning in rats. Three-month-old male Wistar rats were divided randomly into six groups as follows: control (C), sham operated (sham), two kidney-one clip (2K-1C), one kidney-one clip (1K-1C), deoxycorticosterone-salt (DOCA), and N-omega-nitro-L-arginine-methyl ester (L-NAME) groups. Mean arterial blood pressures were significantly higher in four hypertensive groups compared with control and sham groups. The active avoidance training results indicated that hypertension state is associated with learning impairment. Thiobarbituric acid-reactive substances (TBARS) were determined as an indicator of lipid peroxidation in brain and hippocampus. Additionally, brain and hippocampus nitrite levels were studied. 相似文献
183.
Hübner M Dreisbach G Haider H Kluwe RH 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2003,29(2):289-297
Endogenously initiated transitions between tasks are associated with inhibition of the attentional set for the task preceding the transition, as demonstrated by slowed reactions to a task most recently switched away from (U. Mayr & S. W. Keele, 2000). Using an altered methodological approach, the authors found that this backward inhibition counteracts perseverative tendencies when switching to a new task in that it selectively reduces interference exerted by the preceding task set. The reduction of interference was dependent on endogenous preparation for the new task and did not occur for unpredictable task switches or for task switches that were precued without information about the identity of the new task. 相似文献
184.
Pashler H Zarow G Triplett B 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2003,29(6):1051-1057
The occurrence of errors is often thought to impede associative learning. This was tested in 2 studies, each of which involved 2 sessions. In Session 1, subjects learned foreign vocabulary (Experiment 1) or obscure English words (Experiment 2) and received 2 tests (each with corrective feedback) separated by a variable lag. Greater lags drastically reduced performance on the 2nd test. However, they dramatically improved performance in a Session-2 test given 1 day (Experiment 1) or 1 week later (Experiment 2). This pattern held even for items that elicited errors on the 1st test of Day 1. Evidently, the benefit of spacing overwhelms any possible harmful effect of producing errors. The results have clear and nonobvious implications for computer-aided instruction. 相似文献
185.
Schradin C Reeder DM Mendoza SP Anzenberger G 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2003,117(2):166-175
The authors explored whether prolactin is associated with paternal care in 3 monkey species: titi monkey (Callicebus cupreus), common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus), and Goeldi's monkey (Callimico goeldii). They compared prolactin levels in fathers before and after infant birth as well as between fathers and nonfathers. C. cupreus fathers carry infants almost exclusively, have higher prolactin levels than nonfathers, but show no prolactin increase after infant birth. C. goeldii fathers carry infants only after 3 weeks, show an increase in prolactin levels during the precarrying period, but do not have higher levels than nonfathers. C jacchus fathers are the primary carriers, have higher prolactin levels than nonfathers, and show a trend for a prolactin increase after the birth of infants. In conclusion, species differences in the patterns of prolactin secretion were evident and reflect the different paternal roles. 相似文献
186.
Global and local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)
Spinozzi G De Lillo C Truppa V 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2003,117(1):15-23
Capuchin monkeys' (Cebas apella) relative accuracy in the processing of the global shape or the local features of hierarchical visual stimuli was assessed. Three experiments are presented featuring manipulations of the arrangement and the density of the local elements of the stimuli. The results showed a clear advantage for local level processing in this species, which is robust under manipulations of the density of the local elements of the stimuli. By contrast the density of the component elements linearly affected accuracy in global processing. These findings, which support those from other studies in which a local superiority emerged in animals, challenge the generality of early claims concerning the adaptive value of global advantage in the processing of hierarchical visual patterns. 相似文献
187.
The Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ; Smith, Della Sala, Logie, & Maylor, 2000) was developed to provide a self-report measure of prospective and retrospective memory slips in everyday life. It consists of sixteen items, eight asking about prospective memory failures, and eight concerning retrospective failures. The PRMQ was administered to a sample of the general adult population (N = 551) ranging in age between 17 and 94. Ten competing models of the latent structure of the PRMQ were derived from theoretical and empirical sources and were tested using confirmatory factor analysis. The model with the best fit had a tripartite structure and consisted of a general memory factor (all items loaded on this factor) plus orthogonal specific factors of prospective and retrospective memory. The reliabilities (internal consistency) of the Total scale and the Prospective and Retrospective scales were acceptable: Cronbach's alpha was 0.89, 0.84, and 0.80, respectively. Age and gender did not influence PRMQ scores, thereby simplifying the presentation and interpretation of normative data. To ease interpretation of scores on the PRMQ, tables are presented for conversion of raw scores on the Total scale and Prospective and Retrospective scales to T scores (confidence limits on scores are also provided). In addition, tables are provided to allow users to assess the reliability and abnormality of differences between an individual's scores on the Prospective and Retrospective scales. 相似文献
188.
Bielajew C Konkle AT Kentner AC Baker SL Stewart A Hutchins AA Santa-Maria Barbagallo L Fouriezos G 《Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands)》2003,6(4):269-280
The chronic mild stress (CMS) procedure was developed in rodents to target anhedonia, the core symptom of depressive melancholia. Stress exposure has been shown to induce a variety of physiological, biochemical and behavioral alterations associated with depression, although its anhedonic consequences as indexed by either sucrose intake and preference or thresholds for brain stimulation reward are less reliably observed. In the present study, we assessed the effects of six weeks of CMS on the latter measure in two strains of male and female rats subsequently challenged with an acute psychophysical stressor, forced swimming; their behavior in the swimming cylinder was evaluated on two consecutive days. While brain stimulation reward thresholds and response rates were unchanged by CMS exposure, significant differences in forced swim behaviors were observed between male control and CMS groups. In particular, male Long Evans rats with a history of CMS showed the largest decrease in the duration of active behaviors on the second test day, a pattern less evident in the Sprague-Dawley strain of rats, or in any of the female groups. The results suggest that the effects of depressogenic manipulations are strain and gender dependent, with male Long Evans rats most susceptible, as demonstrated by the selective reduction of struggling behaviors. Inclusion of multiple measures, including the forced swim test, would provide a better understanding of the psychopathological profile engendered by chronic exposure to mild stressors and its genetic specificity. 相似文献
189.
Thirty-six university students were tested in a plausibility judgment task using a self-paced listening paradigm under no-interference
and two-digit load conditions. Listening times were longer at syntactically more complex portions of syntactically more complex
sentences, and greater loads led to increased listening times. However, listening times at syntactically more complex positions
in syntactically more complex sentences did not increase more than listening times at comparable positions in syntactically
simple sentences under digit load conditions. The results indicate that a concurrent memory load does not reduce the availability
of working memory resources used for on-line syntactic processing and, thus, provide evidence that the working memory system
used for assigning syntactic structure is separate from that measured by standard working memory tasks. 相似文献
190.
Király I Jovanovic B Prinz W Aschersleben G Gergely G 《Consciousness and cognition》2003,12(4):752-769
We contrast two positions concerning the initial domain of actions that infants interpret as goal-directed. The 'narrow scope' view holds that goal-attribution in 6- and 9-month-olds is restricted to highly familiar actions (such as grasping). The cue-based approach of the infant's 'teleological stance', however, predicts that if the cues of equifinal variation of action and a salient action effect are present, young infants can attribute goals to a 'wide scope' of entities including unfamiliar human actions and actions of novel objects lacking human features. It is argued that previous failures to show goal-attribution to unfamiliar actions were due to the absence of these cues. We report a modified replication of Woodward (1999) showing that when a salient action-effect is presented, even young infants can attribute a goal to an unfamiliar manual action. This study together with other recent experiments reviewed support the 'wide scope' approach indicating that if the cues of goal-directedness are present even 6-month-olds attribute goals to unfamiliar actions. 相似文献