全文获取类型
收费全文 | 330篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
334篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 10篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 9篇 |
2016年 | 13篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 32篇 |
2012年 | 11篇 |
2011年 | 12篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 11篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 10篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 8篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有334条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
The effects of body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio on ratings of female attractiveness, fecundity, and health 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Adrian Furnham K.V. Petrides Androulla Constantinides 《Personality and individual differences》2005,38(8):1823-1834
This paper examines the effects of body mass index (BMI) and waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) on people’s perceptions of female attractiveness and fecundity. One hundred and two participants (51 females) were asked to rate 18 line drawings, varying across three BMI and six WHR levels, on seven different attributes (‘healthy’, ‘fertile’, ‘youthful’, ‘intelligent’, ‘nurturing’, ‘flirty’, and ‘attractive’). Line drawings manipulated arm and leg thickness while keeping torso WHR consistent, thus unconfounding previously confounded variables. The data were analysed through a doubly multivariate analysis of variance. Effect sizes were larger for BMI than for WHR. Figures of average weight and a WHR of 0.7 were rated as most attractive and healthy. Overall, the results demonstrate that the effects of BMI and WHR on perceptions of attractiveness and fecundity are interdependent and should be studied concurrently rather than in isolation. 相似文献
72.
A total of 421 parents from four Southern African countries (Nambia, South Africa, Zambia, and Zimbabwe) estimated their own
and their children’s multiple intelligences. There were consistent country and sex differences in self-estimates. Nambians
tended to give lowest self-estimates and Zambians highest self-estimates. Contrary to previous findings from other continents
females gave higher self-estimates than males on all seven multiple intelligences. These results were mirrored in the estimation
of children: females gave higher scores than males and Zambians gave highest estimates and Nambians lowest. Reasons for these
findings are discussed. 相似文献
73.
The aim of this study was to examine an important pathway suggested by the common sense model (CSM): the relation of illness representations to the 'end-stage' appraisal of outcomes through health status. A further aim was to examine the moderating role of optimism in this relationship. One hundred and six chronic cardiac patients completed questionnaires about illness representations and dispositional optimism at baseline, and health status and illness-related helplessness (as an indicator of the 'end-stage' appraisal of outcomes) at a follow-up interview, 6 months later. Indirect (mediation) as well as conditional indirect (moderated mediation) effects were examined using bootstrapped models. According to the results, the effect of illness representations on helplessness was mediated by at least one of the health measures used. Also, most of these indirect relations were conditional on the values of optimism. That is, optimism moderated the 'illness representations-health status-"end-stage" appraisal of outcomes' relationship, as the mediating effects were absent at higher levels of optimism. These findings can provide us with a more comprehensive picture of adaptation to illness, as well as of the ways the illness-related information is being processed by patients. Thus, they have significant implications for theory, research and practice. 相似文献
74.
Swami V Furnham A Amin R Chaudhri J Joshi K Jundi S Miller R Mirza-Begum J Begum FN Sheth P Tovée MJ 《The Journal of social psychology》2008,148(5):577-593
The authors conducted 2 studies to examine the stigmatization of the female and male body using photographic stimuli of real people. In the first study, 75 female and 55 male undergraduates rated a series of 50 photographs of women ranging in body mass index (BMI) on 3 items: gets teased, lonely, and lazy. Both male and female observers rated bodies on either side of BMI 19-20 kg/m2 higher for the gets teased and lonely items. For the lazy item, there was a clear pattern of greater stigmatization with increasing BMI. In the second study, 40 male and 40 female observers rated a series of photographs of the male body that varied in BMI and waist-to-chest ratio on the same items. Results showed that men and women judged overweight and more tubular men to be lazier, lonelier, and teased. These findings suggest that body size is an important characteristic to consider when examining body stigmatization among men and women. These results also show support for the beautiful-is-good bias. 相似文献
75.
Viren Swami John Jones Dorothy Einon Adrian Furnham 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2009,100(2):313-325
One particular aspect of the literature on preferences for female body shapes has focused on the purported universality of preferences for a low waist‐to‐hip ratio (WHR), despite substantial evidence of cross‐cultural variability in such preferences. In the present study, we examined the effects of manipulating women's profile WHR, breast size, and ethnicity on men's ratings of physical attractiveness and health. A total of 51 African men in South Africa, 56 British Africans, and 114 British Caucasians rated 12 line drawings that varied in two levels of ethnicity, three levels of WHR, and two levels of breast size. Overall, the results suggested that there were cross‐cultural differences in preferred body shape, with the preferred body configuration varying as a function of the ethnicity of the figure being rated. In addition, there was a strong positive correlation between ratings of attractiveness and health. These findings are discussed in relation to the interplay between culture and evolution in determining ideals of attractiveness. 相似文献
76.
Adrian Furnham 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2009,28(4):225-239
In all, 187 participants completed a new, self-report measure of eight multiple intelligences (Haselbauer 2005), a General Knowledge test (Irwing et al. Personality and Individual Differences 30:857–871, 2001), a measure of Approaches to Learning Styles (Biggs 1987), a measure of the Big Five personality traits (Costa and McCrae 1992), as well as gave their own estimated scores on the Gardner (1999) multiple intelligences. Alpha co-efficients were modest with only three of the eight test-derived, multiple intelligence
scores being over .70. ‘Linguistic’ and Mathematical intelligence alone were correlated with General Knowledge. Five of the
eight ‘intelligences’ were correlated both with Extraversion and Openness. Regressions indicated that ‘Intrapersonal intelligence’
was closely linked with Stability and Conscientiousness; ‘Interpersonal intelligence’ with Extraversion; ‘Linguistic intelligence’
with Openness; ‘Mathematical intelligence’ with Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. Correlations between self-estimated and
test-derived emotional intelligence showed correlations ranging from r = .18 to r = .56 for similar type ‘intelligences’. This study provides modest evidence for the concurrent and construct validity of
this measure. It requires more psychometric evidence of validity before it is used. 相似文献
77.
This review paper examines two related areas of research: studies dating back over 50 years on lay theories of the nature and measurement of intelligence, and more recent research on sex and culture differences on self-estimated intelligence. The latter focus is on the nearly 20 published papers on estimated intelligence. Studies have shown consistent sex differences with males rating themselves higher than females. There are also consistent generational effects with adult participants believing around a half standard deviation difference in intelligence with their grandparents being least intelligent and children most. Self-estimated and psychometric intelligence only correlates weakly. Studies looking at self and other estimates of multiple intelligence indicated that participants seemed to believe that intelligence was male normative in that it was specifically those types of intelligence (mathematical and spatial) that most differentiated between the sexes that were themselves more predictive of general overall intelligence. Implications of these findings for intelligence testing are considered. 相似文献
78.
Adrian Furnham 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1989,8(1):30-37
This study examined the relationship between subjects’ actual test derived scores and their estimates of what those scores would be. Sixty subjects completed the 16 PF (form D) and then estimated the scores on each dimension for themselves and another person they knew well. The results showed significant positive correlations on 9 of the 16 dimensions for themselves. The dimensions they were best at estimating were Desurgency-Surgency, Untroubled adequacy-guilt proneness and Threctia-Parmia. Only two correlations (both negative) reached significance concerning their ability to predict another known person’s scores. Whereas subjects believed they were like the other person they nominated (13 of the 16 correlations were significantly positive), in actual fact their test derived scores showed only two significant findings, one positive and the other negative. Results are discussed in terms of lay theories of personality and their relationship to personality assessment. 相似文献
79.
It has been suggested that a central factor in depression is the experience of feeling different from others. This study set out to examine the relationship between pre-attributional consensus judgments, perceived trait discrepancy from others, and depression. As predicted, low consensus judgments concerning hypothetical events and negative self-evaluations relative to others were associated with depression. These results are discussed in terms of related work on attribution theory and social comparison processes. 相似文献
80.
Adrian Furnham 《European journal of social psychology》1982,12(4):335-352
Psychological studies on unemployment in the 1930's and the 1970's and 1980's have concentrated on the psychological impact of unemployment on such things as people's health, self-esteem and social interaction. Furthermore studies have, not unnaturally, concentrated almost exclusively on the unemployed neglecting the employed altogether. Very few studies have concerned the range and determinants of lay explanations or attributions about the causes of unemployment. This study set out to examine differences in the explanations for unemployment as a function of whether people were employed or unemployed, as well as their age, sex, education and voting pattern, The results showed a predictable pattern of differences between the employed and unemployed, the former believing more in individualistic explanations and less in societal explanations than the latter. Whereas there were few sex and age differences, education and vote revealed numerous differences in explanations for unemployment. As in the case with explanations for poverty, Conservatives found individualistic explanations for unemployment more important than Labour voters who in turn found societal explanations more important than Conservative voters. Results were discussed in terms of the psychology of explanations, political socialization and the experience of unemployment. Problems in this study as well as the limitations and difficulties in research of the kind were also discussed. 相似文献