全文获取类型
收费全文 | 510篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
513篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 12篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 18篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 14篇 |
2015年 | 11篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 78篇 |
2012年 | 29篇 |
2011年 | 24篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 13篇 |
2008年 | 27篇 |
2007年 | 16篇 |
2006年 | 18篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 15篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1963年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有513条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
Pepperberg IM 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2006,120(3):205-216
A grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus), able to label the color of the bigger or smaller object in a pair (I. M. Pepperberg & M. V. Brezinsky, 1991), to vocally quantify < or =6 item sets (including heterogeneous subsets; I. M. Pepperberg, 1994), and separately trained to identify Arabic numerals 1-6 with the same vocal English labels but not to associate Arabic numbers with their relevant physical quantities, was shown pairs of Arabic numbers or an Arabic numeral and a set of objects and was asked for the color of the bigger or smaller one. The parrot's success showed he (a) understood number symbols as abstract representations of real-world collections, (b) inferred the relationship between the Arabic number and the quantity via stimulus equivalence, and (c) understood the ordinal relationship of his numbers. 相似文献
132.
Romero LJ Garry PJ Schuyler M Bennahum DA Qualls C Ballinger L Kelly V Schmitt C Skipper B Ortiz IE Rhyne RL 《Journal of genetic counseling》2005,14(2):141-150
The purpose of our study is to assess the emotional responses to disclosing APO E genotype to asymptomatic older adults at increased risk for Alzheimer disease (AD). This is a longitudinal cohort study of volunteer subjects who were aged 50 years or over, asymptomatic for (AD), had a family history of AD, passed a psychological assessment, and participated in pre- and post-test genetic counseling and three follow-up visits over 10 months. We analyzed responses by three emotional constructs: depressed, worried, and relieved. Three hundred and twenty-eight subjects were screened, 76 received their APO E genotype. When emotional responses occurred it was immediate, between baseline and the 1 month follow-up. Emotional reactions did not change significantly past 1 month. Our results suggest that for emotionally stable persons, disclosing results of their APO E genotype, high risk subjects did not report more depression or worry and low risk subjects felt relieved by knowing the results. Future studies should evaluate the risks of disclosure to family members involved in the diagnostic work-up of a relative and include subjects from a broader range of emotional stability and socioeconomic background. 相似文献
133.
We examined grouping under inattention using Driver, Davis, Russell, Turatto, & Freeman’s (2001) method. On each trial, two
successive displays were briefly presented, each comprising a central target square surrounded by elements. The task was to
judge whether the two targets were the same or different. The organization of the background elements stayed the same or changed,
independently of the targets. In different conditions, background elements grouped into columns/rows by color similarity,
a shape (a triangle/arrow, a square/cross, or a vertical/horizontal line) by color similarity, and a shape with no other elements
in the background. We measured the influence of the background on the target same-different judgments. The results imply that
background elements grouped into columns/rows by color similarity and into a shape when no segregation from other elements
was involved and the shape was relatively “good.” In contrast, no background grouping was observed when resolving figure-ground
relations for segregated units was required, as in grouping into a shape by color similarity. These results suggest that grouping
is a multiplicity of processes that vary in their attentional demands. Regardless of attentional demands, the products of
grouping are not available to awareness without attention. 相似文献
134.
This article reviews the published literature on developmental prosopagnosia, a condition in which the ability to recognize other persons by facial information alone has never been acquired. Due to the very low incidence of this syndrome, case reports are sparse. We review the available data and suggest assessment strategies for patients suffering from developmental prosopagnosia. It is suggested that developmental prosopagnosia is not a unitary condition but rather consists of different subforms that can be dissociated on the grounds of functional impairments. On the basis of the available evidence, hypotheses about the aetiology of developmental prosopagnosia as well as about the selectivity of deficits related to face recognition are discussed. 相似文献
135.
Migration can impact on a person's identity and this assault can require reworking or establishing aspects of the self. The study involved a cross sectional design, using both quantitative and qualitative methodologies to establish the impact of migration on women from the United Kingdom (UK) and Eire (N = 154) now living in Australia. A major aim was to determine whether the impact of multiple loss (loss of home, major attachment figure, family, community, culture, and social networks) can cause a grief reaction and threatened a person's identity, and, if so, what strategies might be used to buffer this impact and assist reinvention of the self. Bowlby's grieving process was used as a theoretical framework. Women who successfully reached the final stage were able to reinvent themselves using social strategies, whereas those less able used solitary strategies. Women, unable to reach the final stage of grieving, suffered psychological distress. The study has implications for future migrants. Migrants who use appropriate strategies to enable settlement and reinvention are also likely to achieve a sense of belonging to the new place. 相似文献
136.
Ricardo Basso Garcia Irene C. Mammarella Doriana Tripodi Cesare Cornoldi 《The British journal of developmental psychology》2014,32(1):17-33
This study examined forward and backward recall of locations and colours and the binding of locations and colours, comparing typically developing children – aged between 8 and 10 years – with two different groups of children of the same age with learning disabilities (dyslexia in one group, non‐verbal learning disability [NLD] in the other). Results showed that groups with learning disabilities had different visuospatial working memory problems and that children with NLD had particular difficulties in the backward recall of locations. The differences between the groups disappeared, however, when locations and colours were bound together. It was concluded that specific processes may be involved in children in the binding and backward recall of different types of information, as they are not simply the resultant of combining the single processes needed to recall single features. 相似文献
137.
Pepperberg IM 《Animal cognition》2006,9(4):377-391
The extent to which humans and nonhumans share numerical competency is a matter of debate. Some researchers argue that nonhumans, lacking human language, possess only a simple understanding of small quantities, generally less than four. Animals that have, however, received some training in human communication systems might demonstrate abilities intermediate between those of untrained nonhumans and humans. Here I review data for a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) that has been shown to quantify sets of up to and including six items (including heterogeneous subsets) using vocal English labels, to comprehend these labels fully, and to have a zero-like concept. Recent research demonstrates that he can also sum small quantities. His success shows that he understands number symbols as abstract representations of real-world collections, and that his sense of number compares favorably to that of chimpanzees and young human children. 相似文献
138.
In addition to its primary linguistic function, the Hebrew alphabet is sometimes used as a means of number notation (i.e.,
the system of gematria). Hebrew letters, Arabic numerals, Hebrew number names, and Hebrew letter names were used in a numerical
size comparison task, in which two visually presented symbols were compared for numerical value while irrelevant variations
in their physical size had to be ignored. A size congruity effect, indicated by faster responses when differences in physical
and numerical size were consistent, was larger for Arabic numerals than for number names. The effect for Hebrew letters was
similar to that for Arabic numerals and was stronger than that observed for letter names. These results suggest flexible processing
of Hebrew letters, so that they function as ideographic symbols in an arithmetic context. A distance effect, indicated by
an inverse relationship between reaction time and numerical distance, was found for all notations but was particularly strong
for Hebrew letters. 相似文献
139.
Does intelligence alone explain our achievement at work or in life? Contrary to our belief that academic achievement matters very much in the success we have in working life, Chen et al. (1998) has shown that close to 90% of success in leadership positions is attributable to Emotional Intelligence (EI). Considering that EI can be a potential determinant of our achievement in working life, this study aims to determine if there is any significant difference in the EI of local and foreign undergraduates studying in universities in Singapore. The “Emotional IQ Test” was administered to 100 undergraduates from various fields of studies from the National University of Singapore and Nanyang Technological University, of which 31 had foreign education background. This study showed that foreign undergraduates have a higher EI score than those with local education background. In addition, by examining the relationships between variables such as age, gender, year of study and EI, it was found that males have higher EI scores than that of females. The implication for managers is that staff should be evaluated on their own merits such as their EI rather than on academic results. 相似文献
140.
Recently, Vecera, Behrmann, and McGoldrick (2000), using a divided-attention task, reported that targets are detected more accurately when they occur on the same structural part of an object, suggesting that attention can be directed toward object-internal features. We present converging evidence using the object-based inhibition of return (IOR) paradigm as an implicit measure of selection. The results show that IOR is attenuated when cues and targets appear on the same part of an object relative to when they are separated by a part boundary. These findings suggest that object-based mechanisms of selection can operate over shape representations that make explicit information about objectinternal structure. 相似文献