全文获取类型
收费全文 | 168篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
170篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 4篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 8篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 18篇 |
2012年 | 13篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 5篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 6篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 2篇 |
2005年 | 3篇 |
2004年 | 3篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 1篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1958年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有170条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
This research investigates the formation of consumer attachments to brands contingent upon their attachment anxiety and security. An experiment utilizes a consumer sample and real brands across four product categories (apparel, body care, consumer electronics, and soft drinks) to test the relations between consumer–brand identification and brand attachment dependent on attachment anxiety and attachment security. Consistent with previous research, the findings initially confirm that consumer–brand identification relates positively to brand attachment. Prior work is extended, however, by demonstrating that attachment anxiety has a strong influence on this relationship by moderating the effect of consumer–brand identification on brand attachment but only under low attachment security and with women. Implications relate to attachment research and brand management. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
72.
Franziska Clemens Pär Anders Granhag Leif A. Strömwall 《Journal of Investigative Psychology & Offender Profiling》2013,10(1):125-138
The present experimental study adds to the emerging line of psycholegal research focusing on true and false intentions. It is the first to examine mock suspects' (N = 120) counter‐interrogation strategies in investigative interviews in which they anticipate questions on their intentions. As planning is an inherent part of many intentions, mock suspects were, in addition to questions on their intentions, asked a set of questions that pertained to the planning phase in which they formed their intentions. Besides ratings of the anticipation and difficulty of the questions, participants were asked to provide their principal strategy for being perceived as truthful. Both truth tellers and liars perceived the questions on the planning phase as significantly more unanticipated than the questions on their intentions. Furthermore, liars perceived the questions on the planning phase as more difficult to answer than did truth tellers. Liars and truth tellers differed with respect to their principal strategy employed for being perceived as truthful. Liars' most commonly used strategy was to stick to the cover story, whereas truth tellers' most common strategy was to be honest. The results are discussed in relation to the unanticipated questions approach and psychological concepts such as the illusion of transparency. Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
73.
Prof. Dr Michael B. Buchholz Franziska Lamott Kathrin Mörtl 《International Forum of Psychoanalysis》2013,22(1):50-59
Abstract The four-year group therapy of 16 sex offenders in prison was videotaped, and 21 sessions were carefully transcribed and analysed by means of conversation analysis and analysis of metaphor and narration. These qualitative methods are apt for verbal data and can be combined with psychoanalytic thinking in a productive way. New forms of process analysis can be developed. The results presented here are selected to relate to the topic of how the imprisoned group therapy participants constructed “gender” by ways of speaking about themselves, women, and their victims, young girls. The results show that it would be a mistake to think of these ways of speaking as if they could be ignored in favour of “deeper” motives, lying “behind” the words. Our results show how unconscious constructions of gender are not beyond language, but in language. “Doing gender” is a conversational practice. 相似文献
74.
Clemens M. Lechner Martin J. Tomasik Rainer K. Silbereisen Jacek Wasilewski 《The International journal for the psychology of religion》2013,23(3):185-200
The present study investigated the role of religiosity in dealing with family-related uncertainties (e.g., uncertainty concerning fertility decisions or the stability of family relationships) that arise from current social change in industrialized nations. We hypothesized that religiosity, because it is a central source of family values and norms, reduces individuals' perceived load of family-related uncertainties. At the same time, because perceiving family-related uncertainties may conflict with religious values and norms concerning the family, we expected that religiosity exacerbates the association of these uncertainties with psychological distress. Structural equation modeling with latent interactions in a sample of 2,571 Polish adolescents and adults 20 to 46 years of age supported these predictions. Although modest in magnitude, associations held after controlling for potential sociodemographic confounders. Our study reveals the complex role of religiosity in dealing with family-related uncertainties and underscores the importance of attending to potential downsides of this otherwise beneficial resource. 相似文献
75.
This research used longitudinal data from the German Socio‐Economic Panel Study (SOEP) to examine whether religious attendance buffers the impact of unemployment on life satisfaction. Fixed effects models following 5,446 individuals up to three years after the transition to unemployment yielded two central findings. First, higher frequency of religious attendance was associated with smaller drops in life satisfaction. Second, only those who attended religious services on a weekly basis adapted to unemployment. These results suggest that religious attendance on a weekly basis can mitigate the psychological impact of unemployment. 相似文献
76.
77.
Ellen Collée;Esther van den Berg;Evy Visch-Brink;Arnaud Vincent;Clemens Dirven;Djaina Satoer; 《Journal of Neuropsychology》2024,18(Z1):19-40
Glioma patients often suffer from deficits in language and executive functioning. Performance in verbal fluency (generating words within one minute according to a semantic category–category fluency, or given letter–letter fluency) is typically impaired in this patient group. While both language and executive functioning play a role in verbal fluency, the relative contribution of both domains remains unclear. We aim to retrospectively investigate glioma patients' performance on verbal and nonverbal fluency and to explore the influence of language and executive functioning on verbal fluency. Sixty-nine adults with gliomas in eloquent areas underwent a neuropsychological test battery (verbal fluency, nonverbal fluency, language, and executive functioning tests) before surgery (T1) and a subgroup of 31 patients also at three (T2) and twelve months (T3) after surgery. Preoperatively, patients were impaired in all verbal fluency tasks and dissociations were found based on tumour location. In contrast, nonverbal fluency was intact. Different language and executive functioning tests predicted performance on category fluency animals and letter fluency, while no significant predictors for category fluency professions were found. The longitudinal results indicated that category fluency professions deteriorated after surgery (T1–T2, T1–T3) and that nonverbal fluency improved after surgery (T1–T3, T2–T3). Verbal fluency performance can provide information on different possible underlying deficits in language and executive functioning in glioma patients, depending on verbal fluency task selection. Efficient task (order) selection can be based on complexity. Category fluency professions can be selected to detect more permanent long-term deficits. 相似文献
78.
Franziska Dübgen 《Res Publica》2012,18(1):65-77
Critiques of development aid from its recipient’s sometimes draw our attention to the perception of paternalism on the part
of ‘development industry’ actors. Even within participatory project designs, critical voices recount experiences of clear
power divides and informal hierarchies determining the content and form of ‘cooperation’. While neoliberal as well as neo-Marxist
scholars base their critiques on a distributive scheme of global justice, post-development theory emphasizes respect and recognition
as the central aspect of justice Indeed, post-development theorists continue to complain of neo-colonial power structures
between nations as well as on a micro-level between the ‘experts’ and local people. The latter feel misrecognized in being
judged according to the parameters of Western actors within the international community. This article explores how charges
of misrecognition within development cooperation challenge the assumption by many liberal political theorists that more global
justice could be achieved through more aid. 相似文献
79.
80.