全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2270篇 |
免费 | 26篇 |
专业分类
2296篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 22篇 |
2019年 | 31篇 |
2018年 | 45篇 |
2017年 | 38篇 |
2016年 | 65篇 |
2015年 | 45篇 |
2014年 | 52篇 |
2013年 | 269篇 |
2012年 | 80篇 |
2011年 | 100篇 |
2010年 | 65篇 |
2009年 | 57篇 |
2008年 | 88篇 |
2007年 | 79篇 |
2006年 | 75篇 |
2005年 | 84篇 |
2004年 | 61篇 |
2003年 | 66篇 |
2002年 | 62篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 30篇 |
1999年 | 30篇 |
1998年 | 38篇 |
1997年 | 30篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 22篇 |
1994年 | 34篇 |
1993年 | 20篇 |
1992年 | 29篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 23篇 |
1989年 | 22篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1986年 | 21篇 |
1985年 | 27篇 |
1984年 | 26篇 |
1983年 | 23篇 |
1982年 | 32篇 |
1981年 | 34篇 |
1980年 | 25篇 |
1979年 | 30篇 |
1978年 | 19篇 |
1977年 | 21篇 |
1976年 | 17篇 |
1975年 | 15篇 |
1974年 | 14篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1966年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有2296条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
811.
Joseph Ciarrochi Coralie J Wilson Frank P Deane Debra Rickwood 《Counselling psychology quarterly》2003,16(2):103-120
We examined whether adolescents who are poor at identifying, describing, and managing their emotions (emotional competence) have lower intentions to seek help for their personal-emotional problems and suicidal ideation, as observed in adult studies. We also examined whether age moderated the relationship between competence and help-seeking. Two hundred and seventeen adolescents completed measures of emotional competence, help-seeking, hopelessness, and social support. Results indicated that adolescents who were low in emotional competence had the lowest intentions to seek help from informal sources (i.e., family and friends) and from some formal sources (e.g., mental health professionals), and the highest intentions to seek help from no-one. There was one important age-related qualification: difficulty in identifying and describing emotions was associated with higher help-seeking intentions amongst young adolescents but lower intentions among older adolescents. Social support, hopelessness, and sex could not entirely explain these relationships. Thus, even those who had high quality social support had less intention to use it if they were low in emotional competence. 相似文献
812.
813.
Gerd Bohner Sabine Einwiller Hans‐Peter Erb Frank Siebler 《Journal of Consumer Psychology》2003,13(4):454-463
The processes that mediate the effectiveness of 2‐sided advertising were studied. We predicted that (a) 2‐sided (vs. 1‐sided) advertisements increase perceived source credibility and that (b) the logical relation between the negative and positive product attributes mentioned in the 2‐sided ad (e.g., little space, implying a cozy atmosphere) facilitates favorable inferences about the positive attributes, especially when recipients have sufficient time to process the message content. Results supported these predictions. However, the effects of message type and processing time on attitudes were mediated by inferences about positive attributes but not by perceived source credibility. Implications of these findings for consumer judgment and decision making are discussed. 相似文献
814.
International Journal for Philosophy of Religion - 相似文献
815.
Frank E. Budenholzer 《Zygon》2004,39(2):339-356
Abstract. Philosopher‐theologian Bernard J. F. Lonergan defines emergence as the process in which “otherwise coincidental manifolds of lower conjugate acts invite the higher integration effected by higher conjugate forms” (Insight, [1957] 1992, 477). The meaning and implications of Lonergan's concept of emergence are considered in the context of the problem of reductionism in the natural sciences. Examples are taken primarily from physics, chemistry, and biology. 相似文献
816.
International Journal for Philosophy of Religion - 相似文献
817.
Frank R. Kardes David M. Sanbonmatsu Maria L. Cronley David C. Houghton 《Journal of Consumer Psychology》2002,12(4):353-361
Consumers often focus on the characteristics of one brand and consider insufficiently the characteristics of other brands. Singular evaluation processes often result in judgments of a focal brand that are more extreme than warranted. This research investigates the extent to which this extremity effect generalizes to a consideration set consisting of multiple brands. Three experiments show that a set of favorably regarded brands is perceived as more favorable than would be possible if the salience manipulation had no effect. In Experiment 1, participants overestimated the likelihood that the best brand in the market was included in the presented set. In Experiment 2, the average rank assigned to the brands in the presented set was inflated. Extreme judgments were observed regardless of whether the focal set of brands was offered by a specialty store with a high‐quality reputation or by a general merchandise store with a low‐quality reputation. In Experiment 3, extreme intentions to buy now from the focal store were reduced when a comparative (vs. singular) judgment task was performed, but only when the need for cognitive closure was low as opposed to high. The results suggest that singular evaluation processes lead to consideration set overvaluation regardless of which particular favorably regarded brands happen to be included in the set. 相似文献
818.
The age-appropriate Sport Anxiety Scale-2 (SAS-2; Smith, Smoll, Cumming, & Grossbard, 2006) was used to assess levels of cognitive and somatic anxiety among male and female youth sport participants. Confirmatory factor analyses with a sample of 9-14 year old athletes (N=1038) supported the viability of a three-factor model of anxiety involving somatic anxiety, worry, and concentration disruption previously demonstrated in high school and college samples. Tests for factorial invariance revealed that the three-factor model was an equally good fit for 9-11 year olds and 12-14 year olds, and for both males and females. Gender and age were modestly related to anxiety scores. Worry about performing poorly was highest in girls and in older athletes, whereas boys reported higher levels of concentration disruption in competitive sport situations. Implications for emotional perception and for the study of competitive anxiety in young athletes are discussed. 相似文献
819.
Frank Keller Subahshini Gunasekharan Neil Mayo Martin Corley 《Behavior research methods》2009,41(1):1-12
Although Internet-based experiments are gaining in popularity, most studies rely on directly evaluating participants’ responses
rather than response times. In the present article, we present two experiments that demonstrate the feasibility of collecting
response latency data over the World-Wide Web using WebExp—a software package designed to run psychological experiments over
the Internet. Experiment 1 uses WebExp to collect measurements for known time intervals (generated using keyboard repetition).
The resulting measurements are found to be accurate across platforms and load conditions. In Experiment 2, we use WebExp to
replicate a lab-based self-paced reading study from the psycholinguistic literature. The data of the Web-based replication
correlate significantly with those of the original study and show the same main effects and interactions. We conclude that
WebExp can be used to obtain reliable response time data, at least for the self-paced reading paradigm. 相似文献
820.
Two experiments are performed to investigate how luminance change contributes to prioritized selection of new over old elements. Experiment 1 demonstrates that observers prioritize items that undergo a luminance change irrespective of the direction of that change. Experiment 2 shows that foreknowledge concerning the direction of luminance change signaling the target does not allow observers to prioritize the selection of luminance onsets over offsets and vice versa. The results suggest that prioritized selection of new over old elements is mediated by a general mechanism that is sensitive to luminance change, irrespective of its direction. 相似文献