首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   305篇
  免费   14篇
  2023年   6篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   13篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   21篇
  2018年   21篇
  2017年   16篇
  2016年   16篇
  2015年   5篇
  2014年   16篇
  2013年   39篇
  2012年   24篇
  2011年   12篇
  2010年   14篇
  2009年   18篇
  2008年   11篇
  2007年   16篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   7篇
  2004年   6篇
  2003年   8篇
  2002年   2篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   1篇
  1997年   1篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   2篇
  1990年   1篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1983年   1篇
  1981年   2篇
排序方式: 共有319条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
301.
Journal of Philosophical Logic - We propose a solution to the problem of logical omniscience in what we take to be its fundamental version: as concerning arbitrary agents and the knowledge attitude...  相似文献   
302.
Abstract

This article takes the clinical notion of trauma as a historiographic criterion for understanding modernity and its mythologies. If the rewriting of the term trauma – at the intersection of psychoanalysis and neurobiology, cultural anthropology and philosophy of the mind – is the hallmark of contemporaneity, it is quite acceptable to see the Shoah as a radical historical and epistemological watershed. Never before had such a highly productive bureaucratic-industrial system been designed and implemented for the devastation of the very idea of humanity. Auschwitz was, in the most radical ways, the liminal space between Human and Inhuman. It lay right at the cutting edge of the abyss that the executioner shares with his victim – the annihilation of humanity. This essay crosses a question that has always characterized the social construction of subjectivity: the perimeter of the Human, and the interrelationships between identity, alterity, and recognition.  相似文献   
303.
A multitude of scholars have recently argued that inherently ambivalent reactions ought to be accepted, and our drive toward overcoming them resisted, in order to preserve a reflectively accurate account of oneself. By contrast, I argue that a genuine commitment to overcome ambivalence aims less at avoiding than acknowledging and understanding, as well as possibly resolving, whatever conflicts led us to become ambivalent in the first place. On the other hand, certain forms of ‘reflective ambivalence’ are especially vulnerable to the very kind of self‐deception they aim to prevent.  相似文献   
304.
ABSTRACT

The “Wason selection task” is still one of the most studied tasks in cognitive psychology. We argue that the low performance originally obtained seems to be caused by how the information of the task is presented. By systematically manipulating the task instructions, making explicit the information that participants are required to infer in accordance with the logical interpretation of the material implication “if, then”, we found an improvement in performance. In Experiment 1, the conditional rule has been formulated within a relevant context and in accordance with the conversational rules of communication, hence transmitting the actual meaning of the material implication. In Experiment 2, a similar improvement has been obtained even without the realistic scenario, only by making explicit the unidirectionality of the material implication. We conclude that task instructions are often formulate neglecting the conversational rules of communication, and this greatly reduces the possibility to succeed in the task.  相似文献   
305.
306.
307.
Conditioned stimuli (CS) have multiple psychological functions that can potentially contribute to their effect on memory formation. It is generally believed that CS-induced memory modulation is primarily due to conditioned emotional responses, however, well-learned CSs not only generate the appropriate behavioral and physiological reactions required to best respond to an upcoming unconditioned stimulus (US), but they also serve as signals that the US is about to occur. Therefore, it is possible that CSs can impact memory consolidation even when their ability to elicit conditioned emotional arousal is significantly reduced. To test this, male Sprague–Dawley rats trained on a signaled active avoidance task were divided into “Avoider” and “Non-Avoider” subgroups on the basis of percentage avoidance after 6 d of training. Subgroup differences in responding to the CS complex were maintained during a test carried out in the absence of the US. Moreover, the subgroups displayed significant differences in stress-induced analgesia (hot-plate test) immediately after this test, suggesting significant subgroup differences in conditioned emotionality. Importantly, using the spontaneous object recognition task, it was found that immediate post-sample exposure to the avoidance CS complex had a similar enhancing effect on object memory in the two subgroups. Therefore, to our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate that a significant conditioned emotional response is not necessary for the action of a predictive CS on modulation of memory consolidation.

Biologically significant stimuli (unconditioned stimuli [US]) support learning and promote changes in behavior by enhancing the consolidation of memory (White and Milner 1992). Thus, stimuli such as food (Huston et al. 1974, 1977), pain (Galvez et al. 1996; Quirarte et al. 1998), and various drugs of abuse (Krivanek and McGaugh 1969; White 1996; Leri et al. 2013; Rkieh et al. 2014; Wolter et al. 2019, 2020) increase memory storage and facilitate performance on a variety of learning tasks when delivered during a window of memory consolidation that occurs following a learning experience (McGaugh and Roozendaal 2009; Roozendaal and Mcgaugh 2012; McGaugh 2015).Interestingly, exposure to stimuli paired with both incentive (Holahan and White 2013; Wolter et al. 2019, 2020; Baidoo et al. 2020) and aversive (Holahan and White 2002, 2004; Leong et al. 2015; Goode et al. 2016) USs also enhances memory consolidation, presumably because of the conditioned emotional responses that they generate. For example, CSs that precede exposure to footshock elicit freezing (Díaz-Mataix et al. 2017), avoidance (Dombrowski et al. 2013), analgesia (McNally et al. 1999), as well as sympathetic stimulation such as increases in heart rate (Zhang et al. 2019), blood pressure (Hsu et al. 2012), and release of stress hormones (Feenstra et al. 1999), all reactions that are elicited by footshock itself (Lim et al. 1982; McCarty and Baucom 1982; Conti et al. 1990; Galvez et al. 1996; O''Doherty 2004; Lázaro-Muñoz et al. 2010). Holahan and White (2002, 2004) reported that the memory enhancing action of a shock-paired CS could be blocked by lesions of the central amygdala nucleus (CeA), a region involved in generating the behavioral and neurohormonal responses to emotionally arousing stimuli (LeDoux 2003). As well, similarly to a range of aversive USs (anxiogenic drugs, predator odor, tail shock, restraint stress; Kim et al. 2001; Elliott and Packard 2008; Leong and Packard 2014), the effect of a CS paired with footshock on consolidation was found dependent on noradrenergic activation of the amygdala (Goode et al. 2016).However, well-learned CSs not only generate the appropriate behavioral and physiological reactions required to best respond to an upcoming US, but they also serve as signals that the US is about to occur. Temporal relationships between CSs and USs are learned rapidly during conditioning (Ohyama and Mauk 2001; Balsam et al. 2002), and these expectations modulate the expression of learned responses (Holland 2000; Balsam et al. 2010). Moreover, the ability of CSs to predict USs is heavily dependent on mesolimbic dopamine (DA) activity (Schultz et al. 1997; Flagel et al. 2011), and there is substantial evidence that mid-brain DA plays an important role in memory consolidation (White 1989; Managò et al. 2009; Redondo and Morris 2011; Yamasaki and Takeuchi 2017).This analysis suggests that CSs can impact memory consolidation because of their predictive function, even when their ability to elicit preparatory conditioned emotionality is significantly reduced. To test this idea, the current study used a signaled active avoidance task whereby rats learn to avoid an aversive US (footshock) by crossing from one compartment of a shuttle box to another during the presentation of a warning signal. Miller (1948) posited that animals perform the shuttle response during the signal because it prevents the occurrence of the US, and this reduces the experience of conditioned fear caused by the signal. However, it has been found that avoidance persists even when the warning signal no longer elicits a measurable fear state (Kamin et al. 1963; Linden 1969; Coover and Ursin 1973; Starr and Mineka 1977; Mineka and Gino 1980), suggesting that CSs can promote robust avoidance even though conditioned emotional responses are greatly reduced. The current study also used active avoidance because it consistently reveals robust individual differences in learning (Choi et al. 2010; Lázaro-Muñoz et al. 2010; Martinez et al. 2013; Antunes et al. 2020), such that animals can be distinguished into subgroups of Avoiders and Non-avoiders by simple median split (Storace et al. 2019) on percentage avoided USs. Although the source of the individual differences is unknown, it has been postulated the subgroups learn different behavioral responses to the CS (Lázaro-Muñoz et al. 2010; Martinez et al. 2013; Antunes et al. 2020): Avoiders display a loss of fear responses to the CS as the avoidance response is acquired (LeDoux et al. 2017; Cain 2018), while those who fail to acquire the avoidance response continue to display conditioned fear characterized by freezing (Martinez et al. 2013).By capitalizing on these individual differences, the current study explored whether both the predictive and preparatory functions of aversive CSs play a role in modulating consolidation of object memory using the spontaneous object recognition (OR) task. OR relies on the natural tendency of rats to explore novel objects (Winters et al. 2008) and this task was selected because it has been found sensitive to enhancement by exposure to contextual CSs paired with both incentive and aversive stimuli (Wolter et al. 2019, 2020; Baidoo et al. 2020). Given the evidence reviewed above, it was predicted that exposure to the avoidance CS complex (the training chamber, the retractable gate, the warning tone, and the cue light) would impact consolidation of object memory equally in Avoider and Non-Avoider subgroups. Avoider and Non-Avoider subgroups were tested for reactivity to thermal pain throughout avoidance training and testing using the hot-plate to provide an indirect measure of emotional reactivity to the footshock and/or to the aversive CS complex (Fig. 1). This approach was selected because fear/stress-inducing stimuli such as footshock (Maier and Watkins 1991; Rosellini et al. 1994), predator odor (Williams et al. 2005), and their CSs (Hotsenpiller and Williams 1997; McNally and Akil 2001; Ford et al. 2011), elicit stress-induced analgesia; a well-known defensive response in various species (Bolles and Fanselow 1980; Fendt and Fanselow 1999).Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Experimental design used in Experiments 1 and 2.  相似文献   
308.
This paper explores Bion’s theory of links, L, H, K (Love, Hate and Knowledge) and their minus counterparts, -L, -H, -K, which are not conceivable as simply opposite to or as a lack of L, H, K. Rather, they correspond to a way of experiencing Love, Hatred and Knowledge in terms of absoluteness, and in terms of a radical impossibility of acknowledging loss, relativeness and absence. The theory of links is also examined in its evolution towards the conceptualization of three types of container/contained configurations (commensal, symbiotic and parasitic). These Bionian models are compared and referred to the way Jung articulates the coexistence of ‘good’ and ‘evil’, and the paradoxical nature of mental functioning in relation to the individuation process. The images taken from the Rosarium Philosophorum, particularly the Fons Mercurialis, examined by Jung in ‘The psychology of the transference’ (1946), are explored in the paper. Theory is examined with a particular focus on the adolescent mind and its dramatic phenomenology. Two excerpts taken from the analytic work with a mid-adolescent female patient and a late-adolescent male patient are presented to describe minus-Hate as a form of absolute love, and minus-Love as a form of absolute hatred.  相似文献   
309.
Fruit and vegetable consumption is below recommended levels in the population, paricularly in low-income groups. This study assessed factors associated with self-reported intake and 2 biomarkers (potassium excretion and plasma vitamin C) in 271 adults living in a low-income neighborhood. Attitudinal barriers to change were negatively related to reported intake and to potassium excretion. Poor knowledge of recommended consumption was associated with low reported intake, low potassium excetion, and low plasma vitamin C concentration. Self-efficacy was related to reported intake but not to biomarkers. The authors conclude that several of the psychological factors associated with self-reported intake were also related to biomarkers in this population and that these may therfore be particularly appropriate targets for intervention.  相似文献   
310.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号