全文获取类型
收费全文 | 429篇 |
免费 | 40篇 |
专业分类
469篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 22篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 29篇 |
2016年 | 29篇 |
2015年 | 19篇 |
2014年 | 22篇 |
2013年 | 47篇 |
2012年 | 44篇 |
2011年 | 30篇 |
2010年 | 24篇 |
2009年 | 16篇 |
2008年 | 19篇 |
2007年 | 12篇 |
2006年 | 12篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有469条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
Predictive methods estimating mechanical internal work (Wint, i.e., work to accelerate limbs with respect to BCOM during locomotion) are needed in absence of experimental measurements. A previously proposed model equation predicts such a parameter based upon velocity, stride frequency, duty factor, and a compound critical term (q) accounting for limb geometry and inertial properties. That first predicted Wint estimate (PWint) has been validated only for young males and for a limited number of horses. The present study aimed to extend the comparison between model predictions and experimentally measured Wint (MWint) data on humans with varying gender, age, gait, velocity, and gradient. Seventy healthy subjects (males and females; 7 age groups: 6-65 years) carried out level walking and running on treadmill, at different velocities. Moreover, one of the subject groups (25-35 years) walked and ran also at several uphill/downhill gradients. Reference values of q represent the main important results: (a) males and females have similar q values; (b) q is independent on velocity and gradient. Also, different data filtering depth was found to affect MWint and, indirectly, PWint, thus also the reference q values here obtained (0.08 in level, 0.10 in gradient) suffer a - 20% underestimation with respect to the previous predicting model. Despite of this effect, the close match between MWint and PWint trends indicates that the model equation could be satisfactorily applied, in various locomotion conditions. 相似文献
22.
Maria Donata Orfei Carlo Caltagirone Claudia Cacciari Francesca Assogna Gianfranco Spalletta 《Applied cognitive psychology》2011,25(6):927-932
Cognitive insight is the ability to monitor and correct one's own erroneous convictions. The Beck Cognitive Insight Scale (BCIS) assesses two sub‐dimensions of cognitive insight: self‐reflectiveness (SR), which is the attitude of questioning one's own judgments; and self‐certainty (SC), which is the unwillingness to modify and correct one's own convictions. There is little information about cognitive insight in healthy participants (HP). Here we investigated the socio‐demographic and neuropsychological correlates of cognitive insight in 50 HP who were administered the BCIS and a comprehensive neuropsychological battery. The global BCIS R‐C index correlated positively with age and WCST perseverative errors. SR correlated positively with age. These data support the hypothesis that a less self‐reflective and more self‐confident style is cognitively functional in HP. Thus, what has been found malfunctioning in mental illnesses is adaptive in HP. Future studies should consider a wider range of factors that might affect this complex construct. Copyright © 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
23.
Arabic numerals are diffused and language-free representations of number magnitude. To be effectively processed, the digits composing Arabic numerals must be spatially arranged along a left-to-right axis. We studied one patient (AK) to show that left neglect, after right hemisphere damage, can selectively impair the computation of the spatial frames underpinning recognition and understanding of Arabic numerals, without impairing the spatial frames for coding alphabetic strings or for coding environmental spatial information. The presence in our brain of these specific and precise spatial frames must be rooted in the paramount importance of Arabic numerals processing in our everyday activities. 相似文献
24.
The work presents a psychodynamic, semiotic and dialogical model of affect and of the role it plays in sensemaking. The model is based on the following three general assumptions.A) Affective semiosis works in accordance to the dynamic unconscious which Freud conceptualized in terms of primary process. According to this interpretation, the unconscious is the mind’s homogenising way of functioning, which transforms every categorical relation into a relation of identity. B) Affect is the generalized, homogenising and absolutizing embodied basic intersubjective meanings according to which experience is interpreted. C) Affective semiosis performs both the grounding and regulative functions in sensemaking, orienting the way people interpret experience. 相似文献
25.
26.
Francesca Fantini 《Journal of personality assessment》2016,98(6):576-584
Despite recent advances in models and instruments to understand the role of a client's cultural background, clinical psychologists are not immune to implicit cultural biases that are potentially damaging to the therapeutic alliance. In this article, I present a Therapeutic Assessment with a young Sicilian woman conducted in a university-based student clinic in Italy. During the assessment, I assumed that because we were both Italians, my client shared my perspective (northern Italian) about family and individual values, which resulted in a therapeutic impasse when I responded on the basis of my individual and culturally shaped view of interpersonal and family relationships without appreciating important differences between my own and my client's microcultures. To overcome the impasse, I had to openly acknowledge such differences and reorient myself to my client's goals. I discuss the core processes involved in such a repair in the context of a cross-cultural psychological assessment. 相似文献
27.
Laura Sagliano Francesca D'Olimpio Ilaria Taglialatela Scafati Luigi Trojano 《Cognition & emotion》2016,30(7):1344-1351
Mechanisms underlying attentional biases towards threat (ABTs), such as attentional avoidance and difficulty of disengagement, are still unclear. To address this issue, we recorded participants' eye movements during a dot detection task in which threatening or neutral stimuli served as peripheral cues. We evaluated response times (RTs) in trials where participants looked at the central fixation cross (not at the cues), as they were required, and number and duration of (unwanted) fixations towards threatening or neutral cues; in all analyses trait anxiety was treated as a covariate. Difficulty in attentional disengagement (longer RTs) was found when peripheral threatening stimuli were presented for 100?ms. Moreover, we observed significantly shorter (unwanted) fixations on threatening than on neutral peripheral stimuli, compatible with an avoidance bias, for longer presentation times. These findings demonstrate that, independent of trait anxiety levels, disengagement bias occurs without eye movements, whereas eye movements are implied in threat avoidance. 相似文献
28.
29.
Although rats are able to build complex spatial representations of their surroundings during exploration, the nature of the
encoded information is still a matter for debate. In particular, it is not well established if rats can process the topological
structure of the environment in such a way that they are aware of the connections existing between remote places. Here, rats
were first exposed for four 5-min trials to a complex environment divided into several sectors that were separated by doors
allowing either unrestricted or restricted access to other sectors. In the fifth test trial, we measured the behavior of the
animals while they explored the same environment in which, however, they faced changes that either altered or did not alter
the topological structure of the environment. In experiment 1, closing previously opened doors prevented the rat from having
direct access between corresponding sectors. In experiment 2, opening previously closed doors allowed direct access between
sectors that had not been directly accessible. In each experiment, control doors allowed us to discard the mere influence
of door manipulation. We compared the rats’ exploratory behavior in response to door manipulations that either strongly altered
or did not alter the ability to commute between sectors and found evidence that the animals displayed differential reactions
to the two types of door manipulations. This implies that during exploration rats build a precise map of the connectivity
of space that can be flexibly updated and used for efficient navigation. 相似文献
30.
Lancioni GE Singh NN O'Reilly MF Sigafoos J Ricci I Buonocunto F Sacco V 《Perceptual and motor skills》2012,114(2):353-362
This study assessed a new microswitch arrangement for eyelid responses using an optic sensor placed above the cheekbone and a small sticker on the person's eyelid. This new arrangement, which was designed to avoid interference of the microswitch with the person's visual functioning, was tested on three adults with acquired brain injury and multiple (consciousness, communication, and motor) disabilities. The study was carried out according to a non-concurrent multiple baseline design across participants. Data showed the new microswitch arrangement was suitable for all three participants, who increased their responding during the intervention phase of the study when their responses allowed them to access preferred stimulation. Practical implications of the findings are discussed. 相似文献