全文获取类型
收费全文 | 766篇 |
免费 | 14篇 |
专业分类
780篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 15篇 |
2020年 | 27篇 |
2019年 | 27篇 |
2018年 | 35篇 |
2017年 | 50篇 |
2016年 | 31篇 |
2015年 | 20篇 |
2014年 | 31篇 |
2013年 | 94篇 |
2012年 | 61篇 |
2011年 | 40篇 |
2010年 | 28篇 |
2009年 | 30篇 |
2008年 | 25篇 |
2007年 | 38篇 |
2006年 | 25篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 18篇 |
2003年 | 22篇 |
2002年 | 24篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有780条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
A single lever, discrete-trials observing procedure was used with stumptailed monkeys (Macaca arctoides). Lever-presses during a trial produced colored key lights (IS+ and IS?) which signaled whether the trial would end with response-independent food or without food. During the baseline period, both IS+ and IS? were produced on a variable-interval (VI) 15-sec schedule which began operating at the onset of the trial. The two experimental conditions involved a combination of this VI schedule and a DRL schedule. In one of these conditions, only a response that both met the VI requirement and was preceded by at least 6 sec of nonresponding could produce IS? on nonfood trials, while the schedule for IS+ on food trials remained VI 15 sec. In the other experimental condition, the schedules for producing the two stimuli were the reverse. All subjects eventually learned to produce either IS+ or IS? on the combined VI-DRL schedule. These data support an information hypothesis of observing in monkeys and contrast with data from pigeons which support a conditioned reinforcement hypothesis. 相似文献
42.
Donald E. Henson H. B. Rubin Claudia Henson 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1979,12(4):701-711
Eight adult women volunteers viewed the same erotic film in two different sessions. Their resulting genital responses were recorded simultaneously by three different measures: vaginal pressure pulse, vaginal blood volume, and labial temperature change. During the erotic stimulation, the vaginal pressure pulse and labial responses always increased, and the vaginal blood volume response increased on all but two occasions. Although there was considerable intersubject variability in each genital measure, all three measures were found to have some intrasubject consistency over sessions with respect to either their response amplitudes or patterns, with labial temperature being the most consistent on both parameters. The relationship between the response patterns of the three measures during the film was also relatively consistent across sessions, as was the correspondence between subjective ratings of arousal and both vaginal pressure pulse and labial responses but not vaginal blood volume response. To overcome the problem of considerable intrasubject variability of response amplitudes, it was suggested that the inclusion in the data analysis of several parameters of response patterns, which were relatively stable over sessions, might facilitate the evaluation of a treatment. 相似文献
43.
44.
Claudia Card Jacquelynne S. Eccles Adrienne L. Zihlman Mary Lou Wylie 《Sex roles》1984,10(9-10):837-843
45.
Neil R. Bockian Angela Lee Claudia S. Fidanque 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2003,10(4):307-313
A substantial body of research has found that spinal cord injury (SCI) patients have relatively high degrees of energy, impulsivity, and risk-taking (W. Fordyce, 1964; G. P. Taylor, 1970; B. Woodbury, 1978). The present study endeavors to extend our knowledge to the personality disorder (PD) domain. Forty SCI participants and 40 matched controls were given a semistructured diagnostic interview and a self-report personality disorder instrument. Findings indicated that 27.5% of SCI patients, and a similar number of controls, have PDs. Unexpectedly, impulsive/externalizing disorders (histrionic, narcissistic, antisocial, and borderline) were not unusually high in SCI patients, and were not higher than controls. Avoidant and depressive disorders were unexpectedly high. 相似文献
46.
Rossi G Van den Brande I Tobac A Sloore H Hauben C 《Journal of personality disorders》2003,17(4):330-340
The MCMI-III personality disorder scales (Millon, 1994) were empirically validated in a sample of prisoners, psychiatric inpatients, and outpatients (N = 477). The scale intercorrelations were congruent with those obtained by Millon, Davis, and Millon (1997). We conclude that our Flemish/Dutch version shows no significant differences with the original version of the MCMI-III as far as intercorrelations are concerned. Convergent validity of the MCMI-III personality disorder scales was evaluated by the correlational data between the MCMI-III personality disorder scales and the MMPI-2 clinical (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, & Kaemmer, 1989) and personality disorder (Somwaru & Ben-Porath, 1995) scales. Improved convergence was obtained compared with previous versions of the MCMI-I. Only the compulsive MCMI-III personality disorder scale remains problematic. The scale even showed negative correlations with some of the related clinical scales and with the corresponding personality disorder scales of the MMPI-2. 相似文献
47.
Claudia Carello Paula Fitzpatrick M. T. Turvey 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1992,51(6):580-598
Knowing about the properties of objects by wielding them and knowing about the distances of surfaces by striking them with objects as probes are examples of dynamic or effortful touch. Six experiments focused on the invariant mechanical parameters that couple the time-varying states (displacements, velocities) of hand-held rods to the time-varying torques and forces imposed upon them by wielding and probing. There were three major conclusions. First, when a probe is wielded without contact, perceived probe length is a function of the probe’s rotational inertia; however, with contact, perceived probe length is affected by the rotational inertia and the distance of the point of contact from the probe’s center of percussion. Second, when a surface is struck with a probe, perceived surface distance is affected by the probe’s rotational inertia and the angle of inclination of the probe at contact. Third, under seemingly identical conditions of probing, either probe length or surface distance can be perceived selectively without confusion. Results were discussed in terms of haptic information, haptic attention, and the dynamics of probing. 相似文献
48.
Claudia J. Haferkamp 《Current Psychology》1994,13(3):248-262
Relationships among relationship beliefs, self-monitoring and conflict behavior were examined in 74 married couples. It was
predicted that dysfunctional relationship beliefs would be negatively correlated with marital satisfaction and that high self-monitoring
spouses would endorse more dysfunctional relationship beliefs and display more disagreeing and leadership conflict behavior.
Dysfunctional beliefs were negatively correlated with satisfaction but low and high self-monitors did not differ in their
conflict behavior. Discriminant analyses indicated that marital distress was a function of endorsement of dysfunctional relationship
beliefs; decreased satisfaction with decisions; increased conflict relevance; and increased impression management reflected
in high self-monitoring orientations among husbands and wives’ beliefs that their husbands were not behaving “normally.” The
contribution of gender and impression management to marital distress is discussed.
This study was funded by a University of Wisconsin-Madison faculty research grant awarded to the author. 相似文献
49.
Robert W. Isenhower Michael J. Richardson Claudia Carello Reuben M. Baron Kerry L. Marsh 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2010,17(3):342-347
Understanding the physical and interpersonal constraints that afford cooperation during real-world tasks requires consideration
of the fit between the environment and task-relevant dimensions of coactors and the coactors’ fit with each other. In the
present study, we examined how cooperation can emerge during ongoing interaction using the simple task of two actors’ moving
long wooden planks. The system dynamics showed hysteresis: A past-action mode persisted when both solo and joint actions were
possible. Moreover, pairs whose arm spans were both short, both long, or mismatched made action-mode transitions at similar
points, when scaled by a relational measure. The relational measure of plank length to arm span was dictated by the pair member
with the shorter arm span, who, thus, had a greater need to cooperate during the task. The results suggest that understanding
affordances for cooperation requires giving more consideration to constraints imposed by the fit between coactors’ action
capabilities. 相似文献
50.