全文获取类型
收费全文 | 445篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
452篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 8篇 |
2022年 | 10篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 17篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 16篇 |
2015年 | 17篇 |
2014年 | 18篇 |
2013年 | 49篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 38篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 13篇 |
2008年 | 22篇 |
2007年 | 20篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 17篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有452条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
261.
262.
Some studies with children have shown that there is no semantic priming at short stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) in lexical decision and naming tasks for homographs. The predictions of spreading activation theories might explain this missing effect. There may be differences in children's and adults' memory structures. We have explored this hypothesis. The development of memory structure representations for homographs was measured by a Pathfinder algorithm. In Experiment 1, the three dependent variables were: the number of links in the network, closeness measures (C), and distances between nodes. Results revealed developmental differences in network structure representations in adults and children. In Experiment 2, results revealed that these differences were not due to the cohort effect. In Experiment 3, the relationship between associative strength, as measured by associative norms, and distances, as measured by Pathfinder algorithm, was explored. The results of these three experiments and empirical research from semantic priming experiments show that these differences in memory structure representations could be one of the sources of the missing semantic priming effect in children. 相似文献
263.
Fernando Marmolejo-Ramos Pedro R. Montoro María Rosa Elosúa María José Contreras William Alejandro Jiménez-Jiménez 《Cognitive processing》2014,15(3):253-267
Several experimental studies have shown that there exists an association between emotion words and the vertical spatial axis. However, the specific conditions under which this conceptual–physical interaction emerges are still unknown, and no study has been devised to test whether longer linguistic units than words can lead to a mapping of emotions on vertical space. In Experiment 1, Spanish and Colombian participants performed a representative verbal emotional contexts production task (RVEC task) requiring participants to produce RVEC for the emotions of joy, sadness, surprise, anger, fear, and disgust. The results showed gender and cultural differences regarding the average number of RVEC produced. The most representative contexts of joy and sadness obtained in Experiment 1 were used in Experiment 2 in a novel spatial–emotional congruency verification task (SECV task). After reading a sentence, the participants had to judge whether a probe word, displayed in either a high or low position on the screen, was congruent or incongruent with the previous sentence. The question was whether the emotion induced by the sentence could modulate the responses to the probes as a function of their position in a vertical axis by means of a metaphorical conceptual–spatial association. Overall, the results indicate that a mapping of emotions on vertical space can occur for linguistic units larger than words, but only when the task demands an explicit affective evaluation of the target. 相似文献
264.
Current advances in test development theory have mostly been influenced by item response theory. Notwithstanding this, classical
test theory still plays a major part in the development of tests for applied educational and behavioral research. This article
describes ViSta-CITA, a computer program that implements a set of classical item and test analysis methods that incorporate
innovative graphics whose aim is to provide deeper insight into analysis results. Such an aim is achieved through the SpreadPlot,
a graphical method designed to display multiple, simultaneous, interactive views of the analysis results. It behaves on a
dynamic basis, so that users’ changes (e.g., selecting a subset of items) are automatically updated in the graphical windows
showing the analysis results. Moreover, ViSta-CITA is freely available, and its code is open to modifications or additions
by the user. Features such as these constitute useful tools for research and teaching purposes related to test development. 相似文献
265.
Babinski DE Pelham WE Molina BS Waschbusch DA Gnagy EM Yu J Sibley MH Biswas A 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2011,33(4):420-429
This study compared adult women with childhood ADHD to adult women without childhood ADHD and to adult men with childhood
ADHD. The participants, all from a larger longitudinal study, included 30 women and 30 men (approximately age 23 to 24) with
childhood ADHD, and 27 women without ADHD. Women with childhood ADHD were matched to comparison women on age, ethnicity, and
parental education, and to men with childhood ADHD on age, ethnicity, and IQ. Self- and parent-reports of internalizing, interpersonal,
academic, and job impairment, as well as substance use and delinquency indicated group differences on measures of self-esteem,
interpersonal and vocational functioning, as well as substance use. Follow-up planned comparison tests revealed that almost
all of these differences emerged by diagnostic status, and not by gender. This study adds to research on the negative adult
outcomes of ADHD and demonstrates that the outcomes of men and women with childhood ADHD are relatively similar. 相似文献
266.
Benítez Muñoz JL Pichardo Martínez Mdel C García Berbén T Fernández Cabezas M Justicia Justicia F Fernández de Haro E 《Psicothema》2011,23(2):314-321
Social competence and antisocial behavior in children are interesting variables for researchers and educators. Nonetheless, there are few assessment instruments capable of measuring the two constructs in small children. The aim of this study is to verify the structural validity of the Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scale for Teachers and Caregivers (PKBS-2), in order to determine the theoretical model that best fits the data from a Spanish sample. 1509 children from preschool education (741 males and 768 females) from 3 to 6 years old (M= 3.78; SD= 0.815) participated in the study. Data was analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS-15.0) and the Structural Equations Program (EQS 6.1). The resulting models of social skills and behavioral problems show adequate fit indexes, statistically significant loadings, and a high internal consistency index (Cronbach's alpha). Lastly, the structural model confirms a two-factor structure: a first factor of Social Skills, comprising three variables (social cooperation, social interaction, and social independence), and a second factor of Behavior Problems, comprising two variables (externalization and internalization of problems). 相似文献
267.
Guilherme Monteiro Gomes Carlos Fernando Mello Michelle Melgarejo da Rosa Guilherme Vargas Bochi Juliano Ferreira Susan Barron Maribel Antonello Rubin 《Neurobiology of learning and memory》2010,93(4):589-595
Polyamines, such as spermidine and spermine, have been reported to improve memory retention through the activation of N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors (NMDAr). However whether polyamine agonists and antagonists alter extinction remains unclear. In the current study, we investigated whether spermidine and polyamine antagonists that selectively block the NR2B subunit at the NMDAr alter the extinction of contextual conditioned fear in male Wistar rats. The bilateral intra-hippocampal administration of exogenous spermidine (2 nmol/site) immediately after, but not 6 h after extinction training, facilitated the extinction of fear conditioning. The injection of the NMDAr antagonists arcaine (0.2 nmol/site), ifenprodil (20 nmol/site) and traxoprodil (0.2 nmol/site), disrupted fear extinction and, at doses that had no effect per se, reversed the facilitatory effect of spermidine on fear extinction. These results suggest that exogenous and endogenous polyamines facilitate the extinction of contextual conditioned fear through activation of NR2B subunit-containing NMDAr in the hippocampus. Since extinction-based exposure therapy is widely used as treatment for a number of anxiety-related disorders, including phobias and post-traumatic stress, the currently reported facilitation of extinction by polyaminergic agents suggest these compounds as putative candidates for drug development. 相似文献
268.
Fernando Martin Y. Roxas Eylla Laire M. Gutierrez 《World Futures: Journal of General Evolution》2013,69(8):609-631
Outcomes depending on various interacting and adaptive parts and actors in a volatile, uncertain, complex, and ambiguous (VUCA) world are challenging to analyze using traditional thinking. Hence, Systems Thinking (ST) can be applied as a tool to aid policy makers in designing interventions that anticipates various ways in which a system might react to policy changes through the practice of integrating multiple perspectives into a causal loop diagram (CLD). We formalize criteria for identifying potential leverage points (PLP) in a CLD. Identifying PLPs enable policy makers to apply ST tools to devise strategies that are capable of addressing VUCA problems. 相似文献
269.
270.
H H Carvajal J P Parks K J Bays R A Logan C I Lujano G L Page K A Weaver 《Psychological reports》1991,69(1):23-26
The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence--Revised and the Stanford Binet-Intelligence Scale: Fourth Edition were given to 51 preschool, kindergarten, first, and second grade students. Twelve of 15 Pearson correlations between Wechsler IQs and composite and area scores of the Stanford-Binet IV were statistically significant. It is apparent that the two tests measure similar, but not identical, concepts. 相似文献