全文获取类型
收费全文 | 621篇 |
免费 | 64篇 |
国内免费 | 157篇 |
专业分类
842篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 11篇 |
2022年 | 23篇 |
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 30篇 |
2019年 | 17篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 14篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 36篇 |
2013年 | 60篇 |
2012年 | 39篇 |
2011年 | 59篇 |
2010年 | 65篇 |
2009年 | 52篇 |
2008年 | 45篇 |
2007年 | 63篇 |
2006年 | 69篇 |
2005年 | 55篇 |
2004年 | 34篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有842条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
How accurate are children when dating very long-term memories? Chinese and European Canadian 8-, 11-, and 14-year-olds (N=344) recalled and dated memories from before they went to school in a memory fluency task. Parents provided verification of children's memories and age estimates. Across all age and culture groups, a telescoping effect (i.e., events were dated as taking place more recently than they actually did) was found for earlier memories (before 48 months) and a reverse telescoping effect for later memories (after 48 months). Older children showed a greater tendency to telescope earlier memories and a weaker tendency to reverse telescope later memories than did younger children. Euro-Canadian children showed larger reverse telescoping than Chinese children. These are the first systematic findings concerning the accuracy of children's dating of very long-term memories. They shed new light on the phenomenon of telescoping and have implications for research on childhood amnesia. 相似文献
92.
田丰 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2013,34(3):77-79
以女性主义为视角,从亲子鉴定中婚生未成年子女权益被忽视的现象入手,解读亲子争执涉及的多元冲突.在分析亲子鉴定法制现状的基础上,借鉴先进国家和地区的立法,建构我国的亲子鉴定法律制度:细化未成年人特殊保护原则、完善婚生推定与否认制度、限定DNA亲子鉴定的启动和实施、配套相关制度. 相似文献
93.
94.
The primary purpose of this study was to investigate whether the theory of planned behavior (TPB) mediated the relationship between dialectical thinking and health behaviors. A sample of 285 undergraduates was tested with a dialectical thinking styles scale, health promoting lifestyle profiles, and TPB questionnaires. Structural equation modeling was used for data analysis. Results indicated that all the three dimensions of thinking styles (belief in the connection, acceptance of change, and acceptance of contradiction) exerted significant effects on TPB constructs. Specifically, the connection and the change dimensions had positive effects on health behaviors mediated by TPB, whereas the contradiction dimension had a negative effect. Model 2 showed a satisfactory fit, demonstrating the influential pathways between dialectical thinking and health behaviors. Implications in issues of health promotion and future research are discussed. 相似文献
95.
Ian Spence Andrew Jia Jing Feng Jonny Elserafi Ying Zhao 《Applied cognitive psychology》2013,27(5):633-643
Auditory distractions can have serious consequences in critical situations such as driving. Mobile phones, radios, media players, and information devices that interpret and produce speech are increasingly common in vehicles, but the threats to visual attention are not yet fully understood. In three experiments, we found that most speech tasks had relatively small adverse effects on the detection of a briefly presented target among distractors across a 60° subarea of the visual field. Although there was a little impact on detectability, moderately difficult speech tasks slowed responding relative to silence. Our most demanding condition—generating and speaking a word beginning with the last letter of another word—had the greatest effects on accuracy and latency, with responding slowed by about 900 ms. An impairment of this magnitude presents a significant threat to safe driving and calls into question the belief that hands‐free voice‐controlled devices are the answer to the problem of driver distraction. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
96.
The microstructure and crystallization behaviour of melt-spun Al88Ni9Ce2Fe1 amorphous alloy and nanophase composites have been studied by means of X-ray diffraction, transmission electron microscopy and scanning and isothermal calorimetry. The diffraction patterns from Al88Ni9Ce2Fe1 amorphous alloys are diffuse, indicating a basically amorphous structure but contain two rings presumed to be associated with quenched-in nuclei. In the cases of Al88Ni9Ce2Fe1 nanophase composites, nanoscale precipitated particles are homogeneously dispersed in an amorphous matrix, and the crystallite diameter and volume fraction are sensitive to quenching conditions. During thermal crystallization, a two-step phase transformation occurs in the amorphous alloy and nanocomposites, which is characterized by a diffusion-controlled precipitation of nanoscale Al particles and the growth of a Al3(Ni, Fe) nanophase prior to a Al11Ce3 nanophase. This study gives insight into structure-control for obtaining nanophases dispersed in an amorphous matrix by rapid quenching. 相似文献
97.
98.
Abstract The parent–twin transformation equations derived by Mügge and subsequent workers have been reinvestigated in view of the observation that these equations produce similar, but not numerically identical, transformations. The current study suggests that, from the point of view of the classical orientation relations, the assumptions made in several derivations are applicable only to special transformation situations. 相似文献
99.
在以旋转的米老鼠图形作为测试项目,对每半岁为一个年龄段共133名3.5一6.5岁的幼儿进行心理旋转的实验研究中发现,幼儿在4岁时已经开始建立心理旋转能力.其发展特征呈连续性;男女幼儿间有一定的性别差异存在,但统计结果提示无显著性意义;男女幼儿在5-6岁间都发现有一个负增长阶段,其原因不明。 相似文献
100.