全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2910篇 |
免费 | 35篇 |
专业分类
2945篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 40篇 |
2023年 | 35篇 |
2022年 | 51篇 |
2021年 | 80篇 |
2020年 | 114篇 |
2019年 | 131篇 |
2018年 | 178篇 |
2017年 | 178篇 |
2016年 | 150篇 |
2015年 | 114篇 |
2014年 | 118篇 |
2013年 | 304篇 |
2012年 | 194篇 |
2011年 | 180篇 |
2010年 | 94篇 |
2009年 | 108篇 |
2008年 | 132篇 |
2007年 | 132篇 |
2006年 | 99篇 |
2005年 | 78篇 |
2004年 | 77篇 |
2003年 | 51篇 |
2002年 | 54篇 |
2001年 | 23篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 21篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 9篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有2945条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
Renk Kimberly Roberts Rex Roddenberry Angela Luick Mary Hillhouse Sarah Meehan Cricket Oliveros Arazais Phares Vicky 《Sex roles》2003,48(7-8):305-315
To examine the relationships among the sex of the parent, gender role, and the time parents spend with their children, 272 parents completed the Bem Sex-Role Inventory, a questionnaire about the time spent with their children, and a demographics questionnaire. Analyses indicated that neither the sex of the parent nor gender role was predictive of the amount of time parents were spending in direct interaction with or being accessible to their children. The sex of the parent, qualified by earning status, was predictive of the level of responsibility parents had for child-related activities. Further, the sex of the parent, qualified by femininity, was predictive of parental satisfaction with level of child-related responsibilities. These results emphasize the importance of examining parental characteristics in relation to the time parents spend with their children and their levels of responsibility for child-related activities. 相似文献
162.
Previous research shows that children have difficulties handling intensional contexts even when they can pass a test of false belief (e.g. Cognition 67 (1998) 287; Cognition 25 (1987) 289). Some authors (Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; Cognition 25 (1987) 289) place these difficulties in the linguistic and not the mental representational domain. The experiments reported here examined whether 6-year-old children could answer questions in an intensional context that did not require the explicit verbal characterization of a belief. We replicated previous findings and found that children answered according to their own knowledge in an intensional context. This occurred even though they responded by choosing a picture to insert into a protagonist's thought bubble rather than report the belief verbally. Children could correctly answer questions about the knowledge state of the protagonist and pass a test of false belief. Further experiments ruled out methodological explanations. Experiment 2 showed that the difference in answering according to own knowledge between the false belief and intensional stories is not accounted for by procedural factors in the two types of test. Experiment 3 revealed that children did not answer according to their own knowledge by default. Experiment 4 suggested that answering according to own knowledge was not a result of pictorial salience. Results are discussed in relation to the simulation-theory debate. 相似文献
163.
Sarah L. Kopelovich MacKenzie Hughes Maria B. Monroe-DeVita Roselyn Peterson Corinne Cather Jennifer Gottlieb 《Cognitive and behavioral practice》2019,26(3):439-452
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Psychosis (CBTp) is an evidence-based psychotherapeutic intervention (EBPI) for adults with schizophrenia spectrum disorders that remains under-implemented in the United States (U.S.). There has been little empirical attention on implementation and dissemination strategies for this EBPI. The Learning Collaborative (LC) model is a method of implementing evidence-based practices across agencies and geographic regions that may facilitate CBTp implementation and dissemination in the US.We applied the LC model in an attempt to enhance the accessibility of CBTp in community mental health settings statewide. Providers (N = 56) from 12 agencies voluntarily participated in an in-person, CBTp workshop followed by 6 months of biweekly phone-based consultation sessions (Phase 1). Twenty-one providers opted to participate in an additional 6-month CBTp LC immediately following completion of the initial CBTp LC (Phase 2). Adoption, penetration, provider-perceived skill development, fidelity, as well as provider-perceived implementation barriers were re-assessed during and 6 months after completion of Phase 2.One year after the completion of the Phase 2 LC, 21% of the original trainee group across 3 of the 12 participating agencies continued to offer CBTp to clients. CBTp trainees were treating between one and two clients each. Self-assessed CBTp skills improved modestly over the Phase 2 consultation period. On average, both clinicians and supervisors reached an acceptable fidelity score on the sessions reviewed. Participating providers identified multiple barriers to CBTp implementation, including features of the training and consultation, the agency, the intervention itself, and psychosocial and clinical challenges associated with the client population.Few CBTp implementation studies have applied a framework to CBTp implementation. The authors adapted the LC model in an attempt to promote adoption of CBTp in community mental health clinics across a large, populous state with poor access to mental health services. Identified challenges and recommendations should be considered in future implementation efforts. 相似文献
164.
van der Kaap-Deeder Jolene Audenaert Elien Van Petegem Stijn Vandevelde Stijn Van Mastrigt Sarah Aelterman Nathalie Vansteenkiste Maarten 《Motivation and emotion》2019,43(5):771-785
Motivation and Emotion - While incarcerated, prisoners are subject to a vast number of rules. Drawing upon self-determination theory, the present study examined whether prisoners’ perceptions... 相似文献
165.
Gender and Self-Reported Sexual Arousal in Response to Sexual Stimuli: A Meta-Analytic Review 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Social constructionist theories and sociobiological perspectives have led to increased interest in gender differences in sexual behavior. This study involved a meta-analysis of gender differences in sexual arousal in response to sexual stimuli. Forty-six studies in which participants were presented with a sexual stimulus depicting males and females and in which participants responded using a self-report measure of arousal were compiled, and 62 independent effect sizes were aggregated. An overall effect size of d = .31 showed a small to moderate-sized gender difference in sexual arousal with men reporting more arousal than women across all studies. There was significant variation in the effect sizes, though, which was only partially explained by variables coded from the studies. It was found that the gender difference was slightly larger for studies using pornographic vs. erotic stimuli, was larger for studies where participants were tested in a private setting or small group compared to a large group, and was much larger for college age participants compared to those who were older than college age. Generally, the pattern of results provided more support for predictions from social influence theories compared to sociobiological theory. 相似文献
166.
Sarah W Bottjer 《Learning and motivation》1982,13(1):44-67
Three experiments examined the effects of complete removal of reinforcement and of the introduction of a novel stimulus on conditioned approach and withdrawal behavior exhibited by pigeons in an autoshaping paradigm. Experiment 1 indicated that complete omission of food from the experimental situation resulted in the gradual disappearance of both approach and withdrawal behavior in subjects that had received either explicitly paired or explicitly unpaired conditioning. Subsequent presentation of a novel visual stimulus reinstated both approach and withdrawal. Experiment 2 replicated these findings in a within-subjects, discriminative paradigm. In Experiment 3, presenting the novel visual stimulus during acquisition when approach and withdrawal behaviors were asymptotic did not produce significant disruption of performance. These data suggest that removal of reinforcement leads to the disappearance of withdrawal as well as approach performance, and that subsequent presentation of a novel stimulys reinstates whatever behavior was dominant prior to extinction—a finding that seems analogous to results of experiments concerning proactive interference. 相似文献
167.
Sarah M. Rieth 《Pastoral Psychology》1993,41(3):169-181
Intrapsychic factors, including the dissociation of childhood wounds, rendered a mandated reporter of child abuse impotent to report a child's disclosure of abuse. The defenses of projective identification, splitting, denial, devaluation, and cognitive impairment prevented her from taking proper action. Passivity trained into her by the church and society reinforced her refusal to act to protect the child. 相似文献
168.
169.
A social indicators community project was conducted in 2007 to monitor living standards and quality of life in Rhini, a low-income
suburb of Grahamstown, Makana Municipality, South Africa. Since 1994, under democratic rule, considerable progress has been
made in service delivery to the formerly disadvantaged in South African society in terms of access to housing, infrastructure,
and a social safety net to mitigate the high rate of unemployment. A representative cross-sectional household study (n 1020)
conducted in 2007 in Rhini found that a positive assessment of the household’s situation and personal life satisfaction did
not reflect better living conditions. Lack of income and employment opportunities appeared to dilute gains from higher living
standards. The project also inquired into attitudes to place names and a proposed name change for the city under discussion
at the time of the survey. It is argued that a place name with which one can identify may be as important as service delivery
to enhance community satisfaction and overall quality of life. Dissatisfied residents who had limited access to services and
expressed less civic pride were more likely than others to opt for a proposed name change for the city of Grahamstown that
would better reflect the country’s new identity and multicultural heritage. It is concluded that a useful pursuit for community
quality-of-life studies in countries undergoing social transformation will be to inquire into the complex combination of factors
that drive perceptions of material and symbolic progress. 相似文献
170.
In this study, we examined the association among perceptions of racial and/or ethnic discrimination, racial climate, and trauma-related symptoms among 289 racially diverse college undergraduates. Study measures included the Perceived Stress Scale, the Perceived Ethnic Discrimination Questionnaire, the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist-Civilian Version, and the Racial Climate Scale. Results of a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) indicated that Asian and Black students reported more frequent experiences of discrimination than did White students. Additionally, the MANOVA indicated that Black students perceived the campus racial climate as being more negative than did White and Asian students. A hierarchical regression analysis showed that when controlling for generic life stress, perceptions of discrimination contributed an additional 10% of variance in trauma-related symptoms for Black students, and racial climate contributed an additional 7% of variance in trauma symptoms for Asian students. 相似文献