首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   325篇
  免费   32篇
  国内免费   88篇
  445篇
  2024年   2篇
  2023年   7篇
  2022年   8篇
  2021年   14篇
  2020年   15篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   22篇
  2017年   18篇
  2016年   11篇
  2015年   13篇
  2014年   20篇
  2013年   34篇
  2012年   21篇
  2011年   23篇
  2010年   23篇
  2009年   15篇
  2008年   19篇
  2007年   26篇
  2006年   26篇
  2005年   21篇
  2004年   11篇
  2003年   11篇
  2002年   5篇
  2001年   1篇
  2000年   5篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   3篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   3篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   4篇
  1979年   1篇
  1963年   1篇
  1957年   2篇
  1954年   1篇
排序方式: 共有445条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
311.
In this longitudinal study, the authors introduced goal orientation theory to the study of cross-cultural adjustment. The authors examined relationships among dispositional goal orientation, domain-specific self-efficacy, and cross-cultural adjustment. Results indicated that a learning orientation was positively related to sojourners' academic and social self-efficacy, whereas a performance orientation was negatively related to sojourners' social self-efficacy. Sojourners' academic and social self-efficacy were positively related to academic and social adjustment, respectively. A learning orientation was positively related to academic and social adjustment, and the relationship was mediated by self-efficacy. A performance orientation was not related to adjustment. Finally, academic adjustment was positively related to grade point average. The authors discussed implications for research and practices.  相似文献   
312.
Recent research has shown that learned fear emerges in a response-specific sequence. For example, an odor conditioned stimulus (CS) previously paired with shock elicits behavioral expressions of fear like avoidance at a younger age than it elicits other behavioral expressions of fear like potentiation of the startle response (Richardson, Paxinos, & Lee, 2000). In the present study, the question of whether learned fear is expressed in a manner appropriate to the animal's age at training or its age at testing was explored in three experiments, all using a within-subjects design. The results suggest that learned fear is expressed in a manner appropriate to the rat's age at training, not its age at testing. The Discussion section focuses on the implications of these findings for (1) the developmental analysis of memory and (2) the idea that an aversive CS elicits a central state of fear.  相似文献   
313.
Past methodological research on mediation analysis mainly focused on situations where all variables were complete and continuous. When issues of categorical data occur combined with missing data, more methodological considerations are involved. Specifically, appropriate decisions need to be made on estimation methods of the indirect effects and on confidence intervals for testing the indirect effects with accommodations of missing data. We compare strategies that address these issues based on a model with a dichotomous mediator, aiming to provide guidelines for researchers facing such challenges in practice.  相似文献   
314.
The purpose of this research was to test, in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, the hypothesis that individuals who use defensive pessimism as a strategy to manage anxiety are likely to act to prevent or mitigate negative outcomes. Across three crowd-sourced samples (Total N = 1416), defensive pessimism was correlated with self-reports of more precautionary behavior (Studies 1, 2, & 3) and fewer COVID-positive results (Study 3).  相似文献   
315.
徐凡 《心理学报》1989,22(1):70-77
本实验分别从运动、照片和“地图”方面对4岁、5岁和6岁儿童在真实空间中的心理旋转及对参照信息的使用等空间表征的发展作了初步考察,结果发现,(1)运动部分客体定位最容易,“地图”部分其次,照片部分最难,(2)心理旋转能力在不同的实验方式下有不同的表现;(3)儿童的自我中心性反应也是相对的,并不一律依存于儿童的年龄,还与客观标记的作用有关。  相似文献   
316.
The sensory match effect in recognition memory refers to the finding that recognition is better when the sensory form in which an item is tested is the same as that in which it was studied. This paper examines the basis for the sensory match effect by manipulating whether a studied fragmented picture is tested with the same or a complementary set of fragments in a recognition memory test (Experiment 1) and in a fragment-identification test (Experiment 2). Assuming that fragment identification is a direct measure of perceptual fluency, we expected identical patterns of results across the two tests if perceptual fluency accounted for the sensory match effect in recognition memory. Instead, recognition memory showed a robust overall sensory match effect (the same fragmented image was recognized better than the complementary image), whereas fragment identification showed no overall sensory match effect (the same fragmented image was identified no better than the complementary fragmented image). Experiments 3 and 4 combined the two responses and showed that the basis for the sensory match effect in recognition memory was a subject’s ability to recognize the matching fragments in the absence of conceptual information (when the test stimulus could not be identified), supporting the idea that the episodic trace of the sensory code is responsible for the sensory match effect in recognition memory. Experiment 5 demonstrated that subjects are able to use this sensory code as the sole basis for recognition memory.  相似文献   
317.
Ⅱ型糖尿病患者的行为特征研究   总被引:13,自引:0,他引:13  
使用修订行为特征问卷调查了112名Ⅱ型糖尿病患者和147名健康对照组。结果表明: 1.男性患者比健康人更加抑郁;2.女性患者比健康人更加焦虑、抑郁和愤怒;3.不论是男性 还是女性,此问卷可显著地区分这两组,越抑郁的人,越不焦虑的人,越缺少合理化这种心理 防御机制的人,越可能是糖尿病患者。结合其它的研究结果,认为存在着Ⅱ型糖尿病易感性 行为特征,主要表现为:高抑郁;低焦虑;对应激的唤醒水平低;不善于使用心理防御机制来 保护和伪装自己;寻找一些有趣的事情:回避痛苦事件;不善于延迟的满足,要求马上得到满 足;注意易分散等。  相似文献   
318.
5—11岁儿童两种空间关系认知发展的实验研究   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
李文馥  刘范 《心理学报》1982,15(2):50-59
本实验是研究5—11岁儿童对面积和垂直这两种空间关系认知的发展。判断相等面积是探讨儿童掌握“等量减等量所余相等”原理的发展过程。结果表明:课题中不同变化因素对儿童认知的影响是不一样的;儿童判断相等面积的认知过程主要可分为直觉判断和推理判断,似乎存在一种由前者发展到后者的过渡形态。儿童处理垂直关系时,除了垂直于斜面和垂直于水平面的形式之外,还有处于这两者之间的一种中间形式存在,这是从垂直于斜面向垂直于水平面逐渐变化的发展过程。两个实验结果都表现随年龄而发展的趋势,其中有两个突出的阶段性特点,即六至七岁发展最快,各项指标差别显著,七至九岁组成绩上未显露增长,在这一阶段中儿童认知易受课题条件变化的影响,各个指标较其它年龄组变化大,这可能是发展中的过渡阶段的特点。  相似文献   
319.
方格  刘范 《心理学报》1981,14(3):27-33
本文是前一同名报告的继续,补充介绍本研究的结果。 本研究的全部实验结果表明:1)儿童对速度的认知是从局部到整体的发展。2)物体运动相对位置对速度认知的影响早于并大于物体运动距离的影响。3)物体运动时间的先后(开始或停止)对速度认知的影响大于物体运动时间长短的影响。4)在儿童速度认知发展中,7、8岁是一个快速发展阶段。5)速度观念的发展是在直观与概念的矛盾运动中进行的。6)10岁左右,儿童形成速度概念的内部条件似已基本具备。  相似文献   
320.
本文综合了对我国十个地区959名7—12岁儿童的协作研究结果。探讨了儿童在认数、数序和系列、数的组成、运算和应用等四个方面的发展情况,并对儿童数学概念和运算能力发展中知识学习与数概念发展的关系、发展的过程、环境的影响、发展的阶段性等方面进行了讨论,也谈到了协作方法问题。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号