全文获取类型
收费全文 | 53860篇 |
免费 | 998篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
54862篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 502篇 |
2019年 | 627篇 |
2018年 | 840篇 |
2017年 | 906篇 |
2016年 | 917篇 |
2015年 | 625篇 |
2014年 | 805篇 |
2013年 | 3868篇 |
2012年 | 1511篇 |
2011年 | 1621篇 |
2010年 | 981篇 |
2009年 | 960篇 |
2008年 | 1408篇 |
2007年 | 1364篇 |
2006年 | 1267篇 |
2005年 | 1141篇 |
2004年 | 1121篇 |
2003年 | 1029篇 |
2002年 | 1099篇 |
2001年 | 1626篇 |
2000年 | 1561篇 |
1999年 | 1169篇 |
1998年 | 616篇 |
1997年 | 504篇 |
1992年 | 945篇 |
1991年 | 902篇 |
1990年 | 919篇 |
1989年 | 900篇 |
1988年 | 869篇 |
1987年 | 846篇 |
1986年 | 876篇 |
1985年 | 943篇 |
1984年 | 755篇 |
1983年 | 693篇 |
1982年 | 575篇 |
1981年 | 536篇 |
1979年 | 873篇 |
1978年 | 628篇 |
1977年 | 543篇 |
1976年 | 552篇 |
1975年 | 740篇 |
1974年 | 816篇 |
1973年 | 820篇 |
1972年 | 701篇 |
1971年 | 636篇 |
1970年 | 611篇 |
1969年 | 689篇 |
1968年 | 806篇 |
1967年 | 709篇 |
1966年 | 714篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
To study malevolent representations, earliest memories were reliably coded on scales of affect tone. Ss were diagnosed with borderline personality disorder: 31 without and 30 with concurrent major depression. Nonborderline comparison subjects had either major depressive disorder (n = 26) or no psychiatric diagnosis (n = 30). Borderline subjects were discriminated from comparison subjects by their more malevolent representations; they more frequently produced memories involving deliberate injury; and they portrayed potential helpers as less helpful. Results suggest the diagnostic significance of malevolent representations, which need to be explained by any theory of borderline personality disorder. 相似文献
62.
J M Gold C Randolph C J Carpenter T E Goldberg D R Weinberger 《Journal of abnormal psychology》1992,101(3):487-494
Effortful and automatic memory task performances were examined in 36 schizophrenic patients and 18 normal control Ss. Tasks included free recall, recognition, and frequency estimation. Patients demonstrated impairment in recall, in recognition, in semantic encoding, and in frequency estimation. Deficits were observed across tasks despite differences in attentional demands. The results suggest a basic compromise of memory function, which is consistent with recent neuroimaging evidence of structural or physiological abnormalities in frontal and temporal lobe structures in schizophrenia. 相似文献
63.
Z Solomon M Waysman G Levy B Fried M Mikulincer R Benbenishty V Florian A Bleich 《Family process》1992,31(3):289-302
While the long-term effects of combat trauma on veterans have been studied extensively, its impact on veterans' wives has yet to be investigated. This study examined the implications of combat-induced psychopathology--wartime combat stress reaction (CSR) and current posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)--in a sample of 205 wives of Israeli combat veterans of the 1982 Lebanon war. Results show that both CSR and PTSD were associated with increased psychiatric symptoms in the wives. In addition, current PTSD was particularly found to contribute to impaired social relations among veterans' wives in a broad range of contexts, from inner feelings of loneliness, through impaired marital and family relations, and extending to the wider social network. Implications of these findings for treatment and further research are discussed. 相似文献
64.
LYMAN C. WYNNE M.D. Ph.D. CLEVELAND G. SHIELDS Ph.D. MARK I. SIRKIN Ph.D. 《Family process》1992,31(1):3-18
This article examines and clarifies controversies about the concept of illness in the field of family therapy. We contend that illness, as traditionally understood in all cultures, is a relational, transactional concept that is highly congruent with core principles of present-day family theories. Family therapists need not buy into a biotechnical, reductionistic reframing of illness as disease. Rather, it is more appropriate to conceptualize and work with illness as a narrative placed in a biopsychosocial context. Such a narrative includes how shared responsibility for coping and for finding solutions can take place, without becoming involved in disputes about causal models. 相似文献
65.
When thinking under uncertainty, people often do not consider appropriately each of the relevant branches of a decision tree, as required by consequentialism. As a result they sometimes violate Savage's sure-thing principle. In the Prisoner's Dilemma game, for example, many subjects compete when they know that the opponent has competed and when they know that the opponent has cooperated, but cooperate when they do not know the opponent's response. Newcomb's Problem and Wason's selection task are also interpreted as manifestations of nonconsequential decision making and reasoning. The causes and implications of such behavior, and the notion of quasi-magical thinking, are discussed. 相似文献
66.
This study examined the career aspirations of pregnant teens enrolled in a program designed to minimize their chances of dropping out of high school. The teens completed a demographic data form that included questions on their career aspirations. The data were coded by Holland type using the Dictionary of Holland Occupational Codes. Trends were discussed in light of the future labor market and critical need for career counseling for this at-risk population. 相似文献
67.
The intent of this study is to apply a cross-level design to examine the influence of contextual factors upon individuals' performance. The results support the hypothesis that peer group characteristics predict individual performance beyond individual characteristics. The implications for using a cross-level approach to study the effects of contextual variables on individual-level phenomena are discussed. 相似文献
68.
This article addresses the relationship between religious affiliation and a very specific form of firearms ownership, that is, handgun ownership. Literature is reviewed relative to explanations of gun ownership. A test of the hypothesized relationship between religious affiliation and handgun ownership demonstrated a statistically significant ( p < 0.03) association. Protestants were found to have a disproportionately high level of handgun ownership compared to other religious groups. Speculation for this finding is reviewed relative to other recent research on this topic. 相似文献
69.
70.
D E Schotte 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1992,62(5):798-800
This article comments on a recent article by Heatherton, Herman, and Polivy (1991) in which they suggest that ego threats are a prerequisite to affectively induced disinhibition of food intake in restrained eaters and current dieters. In contrast, the present review suggests that mood induction procedures that involve no apparent threat (e.g., viewing a frightening film) can also prompt disinhibition in restrained eaters. Thus, any mood induction procedure that does not directly physically threaten the S may disinhibit restrained eaters. It is concluded that recommendations to focus future research on ego threats are premature and may serve to obscure the mechanisms by which changes in affective state influence food intake in dieters. 相似文献