全文获取类型
收费全文 | 30599篇 |
免费 | 1271篇 |
国内免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
31880篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 284篇 |
2019年 | 378篇 |
2018年 | 507篇 |
2017年 | 448篇 |
2016年 | 515篇 |
2015年 | 374篇 |
2014年 | 513篇 |
2013年 | 2319篇 |
2012年 | 888篇 |
2011年 | 894篇 |
2010年 | 579篇 |
2009年 | 570篇 |
2008年 | 798篇 |
2007年 | 837篇 |
2006年 | 768篇 |
2005年 | 691篇 |
2004年 | 667篇 |
2003年 | 587篇 |
2002年 | 620篇 |
2001年 | 1082篇 |
2000年 | 1116篇 |
1999年 | 760篇 |
1998年 | 365篇 |
1997年 | 309篇 |
1996年 | 321篇 |
1995年 | 290篇 |
1992年 | 606篇 |
1991年 | 592篇 |
1990年 | 622篇 |
1989年 | 557篇 |
1988年 | 530篇 |
1987年 | 500篇 |
1986年 | 483篇 |
1985年 | 500篇 |
1984年 | 411篇 |
1983年 | 398篇 |
1982年 | 308篇 |
1979年 | 452篇 |
1978年 | 374篇 |
1977年 | 289篇 |
1975年 | 398篇 |
1974年 | 460篇 |
1973年 | 483篇 |
1972年 | 396篇 |
1971年 | 375篇 |
1970年 | 302篇 |
1969年 | 359篇 |
1968年 | 445篇 |
1967年 | 416篇 |
1966年 | 361篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
992.
993.
994.
995.
Winett RA Hatcher JW Fort TR Leckliter IN Love SQ Riley AW Fishback JF 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1982,15(3):381-402
Two studies were conducted in all-electric townhouses and apartments in the winter (N = 83) and summer (N = 54) to ascertain how energy conservation strategies focusing on thermostat change and set-backs and other low-cost/no-cost approaches would affect overall electricity use and electricity used for heating and cooling, the home thermal environment, the perceived comfort of participants, and clothing that was worn. The studies assessed the effectiveness of videotape modeling programs that demonstrated these conservation strategies when used alone or combined with daily feedback on electricity use. In the winter, the results indicated that videotape modeling and/or feedback were effective relative to baseline and to a control group in reducing overall electricity use by about 15% and electricity used for heating by about 25%. Hygrothermographs, which accurately and continuously recorded temperature and humidity in the homes, indicated that participants were able to live with no reported loss in comfort and no change in attire at a mean temperature of about 62 degrees F when home and about 59 degrees F when asleep. The results were highly discrepant with prior laboratory studies indicating comfort at 75 degrees F with the insulation value of the clothing worn by participants in this study. In the summer, a combination of strategies designed to keep a home cool with minimal or no air conditioning, in conjunction with videotape modeling and/or daily feedback, resulted in overall electricity reductions of about 15% with reductions on electricity for cooling of about 34%, but with feedback, and feedback and modeling more effective than modeling alone. Despite these electricity savings, hygrothermograph recordings indicated minimal temperature change in the homes, with no change in perceived comfort or clothing worn. The results are discussed in terms of discrepancies with laboratory studies, optimal combinations of video-media and personal contact to promote behavior change, and energy policies that may be mislabeled as sacrificial and underestimate the effectiveness of conservation strategies such as those investigated in these studies. 相似文献
996.
Israel (1978) evaluated and discussed research on positive and negative verbal-nonverbal correspondence. In the present report we attempt to delineate the relationship of correspondence training to two major intervention goals-producing (increasing) behavior and inhibiting (decreasing) behavior. The concepts of noncorrespondence and generalized positive correspondence are introduced. Past research relating to the correspondence analyses offered for the two intervention outcomes and possibilities for future research are discussed. The relationship of verbal correspondence training to the issues of response maintenance and response generalization is also examined. 相似文献
997.
J.T. Mayes 《International journal of psychology》1982,17(1-4):65-80
Three experiments investigated performance as a function of the visual hemifield to which verbal and spatial stimuli were presented tachistoscopically. The aim was to relate laterality effects to individual, sex and cultural differences in spatial ability within the framework of a model of hemispheric specialisation. A left-hemisphere advantage for verbal materials was obtained but no advantage for right-hemisphere presentation of visuospatial information occurred in the following samples: British and Ghanaian (experiment 1), high and low spatial ability groups (experiment 2) and males and females (experiment 3). Traditional spatial ability tests had no predictive value for performance on the tachistocopic tasks and an interaction between presentation field and responding hand was interpreted as implying equivalent processing of spatial information in either hemisphere. 相似文献
998.
999.
Raymond T. Garza Gloria J. Romero Barbara Goffigon Cox Manuel Ramirez 《Journal of applied social psychology》1982,12(3):237-253
The study examined the interactive effects of degree of biculturalism and locus of control on leader behavior in supportive and nonsupportive ethnically mixed small groups. Subjects were 36 male Chicano college students who participated in a small group discussion composed of four members: a naive subject who invariably served as group leader, and three confederates representing distinct ethnic groups (Anglo, Black, and Chicano). Behavioral observations revealed several interesting interactions involving group support. High biculturals, in comparison to low biculturals, adopted a more active leader role in nonsupportive than in supportive groups, asking for more opinions and evaluations, and making more clarification statements. Furthermore, while low bicultural externals and high bicultural internals tended to be more interpersonally assertive and to make more clarification statements under supportive conditions, it was low bicultural internals and high bicultural externals who exhibited the most active leader roles in nonsupportive groups. Additional findings revealed that statements made by the Anglo confederate were clarified more often in the supportive condition, while statements made by either the Black or the Chicano confederate were clarified more often in the nonsupportive condition. The results are discussed in relation to previous literature and the need to develop a more responsive social psychology of interethnic dynamics. 相似文献
1000.
Age differences in when (i.e., at input or output) children organize information for recall were investigated in terms of a model specifying that information organized at input is more resistant to forgetting over time than information not organized at input. In Experiment 1, recall of items from categorically related and unrelated lists was assessed either immediately or after a 4-min delay. For 9-year-olds, the effect of delay was comparable for the related and unrelated lists, indicative of spontaneous organization at time of output. In contrast, 13-year-olds showed a significantly smaller delay effect with related than with unrelated lists, indicative of spontaneous organization at time of input. Experiments 2 and 3 demonstrated that, for 9-year-olds, high levels of clustering in and of themselves do not eliminate effects of delayed testing characteristic of output organization, and that when 9-year-olds are biased to organize information at input, delay effects are reduced only when measures are taken to ensure that all the category labels are retrieved. 相似文献