全文获取类型
收费全文 | 40748篇 |
免费 | 836篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
41588篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 433篇 |
2019年 | 501篇 |
2018年 | 742篇 |
2017年 | 646篇 |
2016年 | 713篇 |
2015年 | 469篇 |
2014年 | 665篇 |
2013年 | 3016篇 |
2012年 | 1136篇 |
2011年 | 1223篇 |
2010年 | 697篇 |
2009年 | 804篇 |
2008年 | 1040篇 |
2007年 | 1079篇 |
2006年 | 1015篇 |
2005年 | 879篇 |
2004年 | 862篇 |
2003年 | 815篇 |
2002年 | 762篇 |
2001年 | 1283篇 |
2000年 | 1317篇 |
1999年 | 947篇 |
1998年 | 459篇 |
1997年 | 395篇 |
1996年 | 381篇 |
1992年 | 782篇 |
1991年 | 734篇 |
1990年 | 795篇 |
1989年 | 702篇 |
1988年 | 671篇 |
1987年 | 623篇 |
1986年 | 650篇 |
1985年 | 720篇 |
1984年 | 565篇 |
1983年 | 516篇 |
1982年 | 398篇 |
1979年 | 602篇 |
1978年 | 510篇 |
1977年 | 392篇 |
1976年 | 376篇 |
1975年 | 555篇 |
1974年 | 615篇 |
1973年 | 642篇 |
1972年 | 530篇 |
1971年 | 484篇 |
1970年 | 383篇 |
1969年 | 440篇 |
1968年 | 547篇 |
1967年 | 524篇 |
1966年 | 503篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
Prior discussion of hypothesizing as a session "guideline" has not fully clarified the derivation of hypotheses. Reviewing relevant philosophy of science literature, the authors develop the notions of evidence and evidence-gathering in systemic family therapy. The authors claim evidence-gathering and hypothesizing are interdependent, reciprocal elements of scientific understanding. The structure of evidence is described along with practical and qualitative rules for collecting and using evidence. This model is applied to a case example illustrating the logic of clinical process. Implications are discussed for family therapy training and supervision. 相似文献
132.
Violence and the systemic view: the problem of power 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
P F Dell 《Family process》1989,28(1):1-14
Violence is a strikingly lineal concept that is difficult to address from a systemic perspective. Bateson's epistemological disqualification of the concept of power is often understood to imply a corresponding systemic disqualification of the concept of violence. This position is examined in light of recent feminist criticism. It is argued that (a) violence and power belong essentially to the domain of human experience and (b) human experience cannot be invalidated by theory. Accordingly, it is suggested that the (appropriate) invalidation of power and violence in the domain of systemic explanation should probably be understood both as a deliberate choice that necessarily follows from adopting a systemic perspective, and as a fundamental limitation of that perspective. In neither case, however, should the systemic view be considered to be a valid disqualification of the human experience of violence and power. 相似文献
133.
Defining a family heritage and a new relationship identity: two central tasks in the making of a marriage 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The roles of origin family environmental characteristics and couple consensus-building process within the development of marital relationships were examined prospectively in 16 premarital couples. Three important findings emerged. First, significant correlations were found between dimensions of origin family environment and the reported level of satisfaction within the couple's current relationship. Second, measures of the couple's ability to reach consensus concerning important interpersonal relationships appeared to be important mediators of the association between origin family environment and current relationship satisfaction. Third, important gender differences emerged that replicate and extend prior findings that portray women in the role of "relationship specialist" within their marriages. As a group, these findings were best explained by a developmental model of early marriage that envisions the young couple as facing two entwined tasks: to define both their family heritage and their new relationship identity. Overall, the importance of examining family-of-origin characteristics and consensus-building process as critical determinants of the fate of intimate relationships received strong support. The gender differences found in the literature reviewed, as well as the results of this study, have largely been overlooked by the theory and practice of family therapy. Efforts on both the research and theory construction fronts are needed to increase our understanding of the mechanisms whereby prior family experience is brought forward into current intimate relationships. 相似文献
134.
The Reverend William E. Collins M.Div. F.C.C. A.T.R. 《Journal of religion and health》1989,28(1):70-79
Loneliness as part of the human condition is first explored from an experiential-philosophical perspective. A biblical survey suggests loneliness is normative to the faith experience and may portray a more realistic religiosity. Desert imagery in Scripture indicates a strong association among solitude, spiritual development, and potential dangers of spiritual deception. God's not yet-ness-itself a cause of existential loneliness—is paradoxically the way in which communion with God functions in the here and now. The creative use of solitude may enhance social, psychological, and spiritual well-being, and provide a way to theologize about personal isolation and disorientation.He is also editor of theHarding Journal of Religion and Psychiatry. 相似文献
135.
A.C. Mundy-Castle D.J. Wilson P.S. Sibanda J.S. Sibanda 《International journal of psychology》1989,24(1-5):539-546
The cognitive effects of LOGO were examined among 11–12-year-old black and white Zimbabwean girls and boys. Fifty-two experimental group children were exposed to a BBC LOGO programme with a screen turtle. Forty-one control group children received no exposure to computers at school. Pre-tests on four WISC-R subscales were held in January, 1987 and post-tests on the same four WISC-R subscales were held in November/December, 1987. The data were analyzed with multivariate analysis of covariance, using pre-test scores as the covariate. The results suggested that children in the experimental group did better on the WISC-R Arithmetic scale and that black girls exposed to LOGO showed particular gains in spatial reasoning, as measured by the WISC-R Block Design scale. It is concluded that LOGO appears to facilitate cognitive growth. Methodological limitations of the present study are discussed and suggestions for future research are presented. 相似文献
136.
Cerebral laterality and depression: differences in perceptual asymmetry among diagnostic subtypes 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
G E Bruder F M Quitkin J W Stewart C Martin M M Voglmaier W M Harrison 《Journal of abnormal psychology》1989,98(2):177-186
Examined the influence of diagnostic subtype of depression on perceptual asymmetry for dichotic listening and visual tachistoscopic tasks. A total of 65 unmedicated patients with major depressive disorders and 30 normal controls were tested on a verbal and nonverbal task in each modality. Patients diagnosed according to the DSM-III with melancholia had abnormal perceptual asymmetry for dichotic nonsense syllable and complex tone tasks. In contrast, patients having a nonmelancholic "atypical depression" (reactivity of mood with preserved pleasure capacity and associated features) did not differ from normal controls on these tasks, but had an increased incidence of left handedness. Bipolar depression (history of hypomania) differed from unipolar depression in showing abnormal perceptual asymmetry for a tachistoscopic dot enumeration task. Alterations of perceptual asymmetry in melancholia and bipolar depression were consistent with hypothesized right hemisphere dysfunction. 相似文献
137.
138.
From rarity to evaluative extremity: effects of prevalence information on evaluations of positive and negative characteristics 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Experiments showed a scarcity principle in evaluative judgments such that the identical characteristic is evaluated more extremely the lower its perceived prevalence. In Study 1, Ss evaluated a fictitious medical condition that was described as either beneficial or detrimental to health and as occurring in either 30% or in one half of 1% of a test population. The condition was evaluated more extremely--as as a more positive health asset or a more negative health liability--in the low-prevalence than in the high-prevalence conditions. Study 2 demonstrated the same effect in self-evaluations and with a different manipulation of perceived prevalence. Ss were told that they actually had the fictitious medical condition, that it was either beneficial or detrimental to their health, and either that they were the only 1 of 5 Ss who had it or that 4 of the 5 did. Low-prevalence Ss exhibited more extreme evaluative, affective, and behavioral reactions to the medical condition than did high-prevalence Ss. The origins and validity of the scarcity principle are discussed, as are its implications for uniqueness theory, reactance theory, and social evaluation theories. 相似文献
139.
Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: a multimethod study 总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7
N Eisenberg R A Fabes P A Miller J Fultz R Shell R M Mathy R R Reno 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1989,57(1):55-66
Assessed sympathy and personal distress with facial and physiological indexes (heart rate) as well as self-report indexes and examined the relations of these various indexes to prosocial behavior for children and adults in an easy escape condition. Heart rate deceleration during exposure to the needy others was associated with increased willingness to help. In addition, adults' reports of sympathy, as well as facial sadness and concerned attention, were positively related to their intention to assist. For children, there was some indication that report of positive affect and facial distress were negatively related to prosocial intentions and behavior, whereas facial concern was positively related to the indexes of prosocial behavior. These findings are interpreted as providing additional, convergent support for the notion that sympathy and personal distress are differentially related to prosocial behavior. 相似文献
140.
The structure of interpersonal traits: Wiggins's circumplex and the five-factor model 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Using a sample of 315 adult men and women, self-reports on Wiggins's revised Interpersonal Adjective Scales were jointly factored with self-reports, peer ratings, and spouse ratings on the NEO Personality Inventory to examine the relations between the two models. Results suggest that the interpersonal circumplex is defined by the two dimensions of Extraversion and Agreeableness, and that the circular ordering of variables is not an artifact of response biases or cognitive schemata. Circumplex and dimensional models appear to complement each other in describing the structure of personality, and both may be useful to social psychologists in understanding interpersonal behavior. 相似文献