全文获取类型
收费全文 | 16508篇 |
免费 | 348篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
16857篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 224篇 |
2018年 | 259篇 |
2017年 | 219篇 |
2016年 | 260篇 |
2015年 | 173篇 |
2014年 | 262篇 |
2013年 | 1280篇 |
2012年 | 438篇 |
2011年 | 486篇 |
2010年 | 292篇 |
2009年 | 312篇 |
2008年 | 414篇 |
2007年 | 443篇 |
2006年 | 421篇 |
2005年 | 346篇 |
2004年 | 372篇 |
2003年 | 334篇 |
2002年 | 335篇 |
2001年 | 452篇 |
2000年 | 512篇 |
1999年 | 342篇 |
1998年 | 205篇 |
1997年 | 155篇 |
1996年 | 166篇 |
1995年 | 164篇 |
1992年 | 276篇 |
1991年 | 285篇 |
1990年 | 296篇 |
1989年 | 277篇 |
1988年 | 268篇 |
1987年 | 234篇 |
1986年 | 246篇 |
1985年 | 260篇 |
1984年 | 192篇 |
1983年 | 209篇 |
1982年 | 159篇 |
1980年 | 156篇 |
1979年 | 250篇 |
1978年 | 220篇 |
1977年 | 153篇 |
1975年 | 215篇 |
1974年 | 246篇 |
1973年 | 268篇 |
1972年 | 225篇 |
1971年 | 212篇 |
1970年 | 163篇 |
1969年 | 196篇 |
1968年 | 248篇 |
1967年 | 240篇 |
1966年 | 209篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
Clive R. Hollin Julia C. Houston Maria F. Kent 《Personality and individual differences》1985,6(4):485-492
Studies with clinical populations have shown a high level of neuroticism amongst those with an eating disorder. It is unclear however whether the elevated neuroticism is a function of the disorder, or if it is an aetiological factor in the disorder's development. The present survey of a non-clinical female population revealed that those high in neuroticism gave higher—but not abnormally high—scores on a measure of concern about eating. The high-N group also reported a significantly greater number of undesirable life events; a greater familiarity with ‘diet literature’; increased dieting; but no difference in body weight from those in the middle and lower ranges on the N scale. On the basis of these findings it is suggested that neuroticism is an aetiological factor in disordered eating, at least with secondary if not primary disorders. It is propesed that neuroticism is one mediating psychological factor in a triadic relationship between personal environment contingencies, socio-cultural pressures for thinness and psychological variables. Suggestions are made for future investigation of the role of neuroticism in eating disturbance, with note of potential therapeutic application of such knowledge. 相似文献
992.
993.
994.
The development of hand preference in infancy was investigated longitudinally by using a visually-directed reaching task. Thirty-two infants, equally divided into groups of familial right- and left-handed boys and girls, were tested every 3 weeks from 24 to 39 weeks of age and once again at 52 weeks. Group trends for the development of hand preference were differentiated by familial handedness and sex of the infant. At all ages, test object position (to the infant's right or left) strongly influenced the hand used for reaching. Marked variability both between and within infants demonstrated an instability of early hand preference, an effect that could be appreciated fully only with a prospective longitudinal design. The results thus suggest that the development of hand preference for reaching is highly variable, discontinuous, and related to the interaction of sex and familial handedness. 相似文献
995.
Lotte F. Van Dillen Daniël Lakens Kees van den Bos 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2011,47(1):235-240
The present research demonstrates that the attention bias to angry faces is modulated by how people categorize these faces. Since facial expressions contain psychologically meaningful information for social categorizations (i.e., gender, personality) but not for non-social categorizations (i.e., eye-color), angry facial expression should especially capture attention during social categorization tasks. Indeed, in three studies, participants were slower to name the gender of angry compared to happy or neutral faces, but not their color (blue or green; Study 1) or eye-color (blue or brown; Study 2). Furthermore, when different eye-colors were linked to a personality trait (introversion, extraversion) versus sensitivity to light frequencies (high, low), angry faces only slowed down categorizations when eye-color was indicative of a social characteristic (Study 3). Thus, vigilance for angry facial expressions is contingent on people's categorization goals, supporting the perspective that even basic attentional processes are moderated by social influences. 相似文献
996.
Recent work on social change illustrates that disadvantaged-group members are sometimes less influenced by prejudice-reduction strategies than are advantaged-group members, and interventions to improve intergroup relations (e.g., commonality) can sometimes have the unintended consequence of reducing social-change motivations among members of disadvantaged groups. Focusing on disadvantaged groups' (i.e., racial/ethnic minorities) orientations toward advantaged groups, the present research experimentally investigated the potential of dual, relative to common, identity to produce greater willingness to engage in contact, while maintaining social change motivation. Relative to common identity, dual identity produced not only greater willingness to engage in contact, which was mediated by perceptions of shared values, but also greater social change motivation, mediated by decreased optimism about future relations. Thus, for dual identity, enhancing approach motivation (willingness for contact) does not necessarily undermine social change motivation. Implications for intergroup relations and more broadly social change are discussed. 相似文献
997.
Douglas K. Snyder Molly F. Gasbarrini Brian D. Doss David M. Scheider 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》2011,41(4):201-208
Both the prevalence and adverse impact of infidelity argue for the importance of disseminating evidence-based interventions
for couples struggling with this highly disruptive event. We describe an integrative approach for promoting recovery from
infidelity drawing on empirically supported treatments for couple distress as well as empirical literature regarding recovery
from interpersonal trauma and recovery from relationship injuries. We then describe a structured 8 session adaptation of this
protocol tailored to military couples struggling with issues of infidelity. Reaching these high-risk couples requires extending
both the availability and efficacy of services delivered by professionals within the military from diverse disciplines. We
present initial findings indicating that US Army chaplains receiving training in this structured protocol not only can learn
information regarding the conceptual underpinnings and specific interventions comprising this treatment, but also can apply
it appropriately in analog clinical situations. We conclude with suggestions for extending dissemination efforts to reduce
acute adverse consequences experienced by service members and their partners experiencing infidelity. 相似文献
998.
This paper surveys the field of Judaism and health. The authors trace the history of discourse on health and healing within
Judaism, from the biblical and rabbinic eras to contemporary research and writing on Jewish bioethics, pastoral care, communal
services, and aging, including congregational and community programming related to health and illness and the emergence of
the Jewish healing movement. The work of the Kalsman Institute on Judaism and Health is described, focusing on efforts to
unite these various threads into a scholarly field emphasizing basic and applied research on the instrumental functions of
Jewish religious life for health and well-being. 相似文献
999.
1000.
DeWall CN Twenge JM Koole SL Baumeister RF Marquez A Reid MW 《Emotion (Washington, D.C.)》2011,11(3):623-636
Nine experiments tested competing hypotheses regarding nonconscious affective responses to acute social exclusion and how such responses may relate to positive mental health. The results strongly and consistently indicated that acute social exclusion increased nonconscious positive affect. Compared to nonexcluded participants, excluded participants recalled more positive memories from childhood than did accepted participants (Experiment 1), gave greater weight to positive emotion in their judgments of word similarity (Experiments 2 and 3), and completed more ambiguous word stems with happy words (Experiments 4a and 4b). This process was apparently automatic, as participants asked to imagine exclusion overestimated explicit distress and underestimated implicit positivity (Experiment 3). Four final experiments showed that this automatic emotion regulation process was found among participants low (but not high) in depressive symptoms (Experiments 5 and 6) and among participants high (but not low) in self-esteem (Experiments 7 and 8). These findings suggest that acute exclusion sets in motion an automatic emotion regulation process in which positive emotions become highly accessible, which relates to positive mental health. 相似文献