全文获取类型
收费全文 | 45504篇 |
免费 | 1858篇 |
国内免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
47389篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 443篇 |
2019年 | 613篇 |
2018年 | 823篇 |
2017年 | 761篇 |
2016年 | 884篇 |
2015年 | 581篇 |
2014年 | 763篇 |
2013年 | 3536篇 |
2012年 | 1374篇 |
2011年 | 1424篇 |
2010年 | 846篇 |
2009年 | 940篇 |
2008年 | 1212篇 |
2007年 | 1239篇 |
2006年 | 1139篇 |
2005年 | 971篇 |
2004年 | 985篇 |
2003年 | 906篇 |
2002年 | 923篇 |
2001年 | 1427篇 |
2000年 | 1480篇 |
1999年 | 1064篇 |
1998年 | 507篇 |
1997年 | 437篇 |
1995年 | 445篇 |
1992年 | 870篇 |
1991年 | 830篇 |
1990年 | 818篇 |
1989年 | 783篇 |
1988年 | 771篇 |
1987年 | 696篇 |
1986年 | 700篇 |
1985年 | 753篇 |
1984年 | 600篇 |
1983年 | 582篇 |
1982年 | 458篇 |
1979年 | 692篇 |
1978年 | 520篇 |
1977年 | 444篇 |
1976年 | 456篇 |
1975年 | 552篇 |
1974年 | 658篇 |
1973年 | 701篇 |
1972年 | 599篇 |
1971年 | 536篇 |
1970年 | 470篇 |
1969年 | 510篇 |
1968年 | 651篇 |
1967年 | 594篇 |
1966年 | 553篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
Predicting and scaling hens' preferences for topographically different responses 总被引:2,自引:1,他引:1 下载免费PDF全文
Six hens were exposed to several concurrent (second-order) variable-interval schedules in which the response requirements on the alternatives were varied. The response requirements were one key peck versus five key pecks, one key peck versus one door push, and five key pecks versus one door push. Response- and time-allocation ratios undermatched the obtained reinforcement ratios but were well described by the generalized matching law. Time and response bias estimates from two pairs of response requirements were used to predict bias in the third pairing. The predicted values were close to those obtained; this result supports the notion that both numerically and topographically different responses act as constant sources of bias within the generalized matching law. The differences between the response and time biases could be accounted for by the different times needed to complete each response requirement. The results also suggest that the door push is a useful operant for research with domestic hens. 相似文献
992.
Peppin JF 《Journal of religion and health》1995,34(4):287-300
For years articles have decried the lack of empathy in physicians' relationships with patients. In addition to being empathetic, physicians are called upon to assume the posture of value neutrality, i.e., not imposing one's values when dealing with patients. Empathy is clearly an expression of deeply held values; even the language used to define it is value-laden. Physicians are consistently called upon to exhibit traits which are expressions of their underlying values. However, if proponents of value-neutrality are to be taken literally one must not impose any of one's values on one's patients. But then one wonders how empathy could ever be expressed; further, it is hard to imagine what a truly value neutral physician would be like. It is time we recognize that any relationship between two persons requires the expression of values from both parties. These values help shape that relationship and define its further history. Physicians are not excluded from this process just because they consider themselves professionals. Divulging personal values to patients is both more honest and more in keeping with the concepts of justice, beneficence, non maleficence, and autonomy than to feign value-neutrality. A presentation of some type statement of values is timely and would give patients an idea of where a physician stands on a myriad of issues. Rather than being value-neutral, value non-neutrality seems a more right and reasonable posture for physicians to assume. 相似文献
993.
In this study, we explored differences in personality and daily life experiences of traumatized (n= 26) versus nontraumatized (n= 30) college students. Study participants completed a variety of personality measures as well as a 28–day experience sampling study assessing daily activities, emotions, and physical health. Although not differing on general demographics, traumatized individuals reported more trait anxiety and lower self–esteem than nontraumatized individuals. They scored higher on Neuroticism, were more introverted, and were less emotionally stable than nontraumatized participants. Traumatized individuals also reported more cognitive disturbances, emotional blunting, and interpersonal withdrawal. They did not report being more depressed, but did endorse cognitive styles associated with heightened risk for depression. Earlier age of trauma was associated with more pathological outcomes: lower self–esteem and psychological well–being, more anxiety, more pessimism, and emotional constriction of positive mood. We compare this symptom profile to that of posttraumatic stress disorder. 相似文献
994.
995.
Todd Q. Miller C. David Jenkins George A. Kaplan Jukka T. Salonen 《Journal of applied social psychology》1995,25(13):1142-1168
Few studies have examined the correlational and structural relationships between measures of hostility that may predict coronary heart disease. Using data from a population-based study (N= 2,682), we examined the factor structure among the five subscales from Siegel's (1986) Multi-dimensional Anger Inventory, the three subscales from Spielberger et al.'s (1985) anger expression scales, a Cook-Medley (Cook & Madley, 1954) hostility subscale that measures cynicism, and items measuring hostility from four scales that assess Type A behavior. Eight separate factors were identified: hostile anger expression, perceived control over the expression of one's anger, frequency of anger, ease of anger provocation, brooding, hostile outlook, cynicism, and sullenness. These results suggest that the structure of hostility measures is more complex than previous factor analyses have suggested. Our findings point to the dangers in assuming that a factor structure can be generalized beyond the data on which it is based to provide theoretical statements about psychological structure. The new scales appear to be valid because they correlate with demographic variables and indicators of social support in the expected directions. 相似文献
996.
997.
998.
Toussaint HM Commissaris DA Van Dieeumlant;n JH Reijnen JS Praet SF Beek PJ 《Journal of motor behavior》1995,27(3):225-234
The control of the ground reaction force vector relative to the center of gravity (CoG) was examined while subjects performed a back-lifting task. Six male subjects (aged 24.0 +/- 2.5 years) repeatedly lifted a barbell. A biomechanical analysis that used a linked segment model revealed that the summed rotations of body segments during lifting yielded a specific rate of change of the angular momentum of the entire body. This equaled the external moment provided by Fsubg; relative to CoG. This implies that multisegment movements involve control of the angular momentum of the entire body through an appropriately directed Fsubg;. Thus, in dynamic tasks Fsubg; is pointed away from rather than lined up with the CoG, as is the case in static tasks. 相似文献
999.
Elena F. Brand Brian Lakey Shirley Berman 《American journal of community psychology》1995,23(1):117-135
Investigated the effects of a 13-week preventive, psychoeducational intervention program to improve perceived social support.
Fifty-one, low-perceived support, community residents were randomly assigned to an intervention or wait-list control condition.
Intervention subjects received training in social skills and cognitive reframing regarding the self and social relations.
The intervention led to increased perceived social support from family, but not from friends. As hypothesized by social cognition
models, increases in perceived support appeared to be mediated by changes in self-esteem and frequency of self-reinforcement.
Further, such changes in cognition about the self were larger than the changes observed for perceived support, suggesting
that it may be easier to change cognition about the self than perceptions of support. 相似文献
1000.