全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2908篇 |
免费 | 139篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
3049篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 43篇 |
2022年 | 43篇 |
2021年 | 51篇 |
2020年 | 79篇 |
2019年 | 58篇 |
2018年 | 118篇 |
2017年 | 127篇 |
2016年 | 115篇 |
2015年 | 93篇 |
2014年 | 92篇 |
2013年 | 322篇 |
2012年 | 187篇 |
2011年 | 136篇 |
2010年 | 94篇 |
2009年 | 94篇 |
2008年 | 124篇 |
2007年 | 134篇 |
2006年 | 80篇 |
2005年 | 88篇 |
2004年 | 75篇 |
2003年 | 61篇 |
2002年 | 55篇 |
2001年 | 63篇 |
2000年 | 61篇 |
1999年 | 44篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 13篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 30篇 |
1989年 | 26篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 27篇 |
1986年 | 22篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 27篇 |
1976年 | 14篇 |
1975年 | 20篇 |
1974年 | 23篇 |
1973年 | 18篇 |
1972年 | 14篇 |
1971年 | 16篇 |
1970年 | 19篇 |
1968年 | 18篇 |
排序方式: 共有3049条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
Zusammenfassung
Die Transaktionsanalyse (Berne 1958) sollte vom ursprünglichen Konzept her dazu dienen, personale Strukturen und psychodynamische
Prozesse auch dem Laien verstehbar und nutzbar zu machen. Obwohl dieser Anspruch bei manchen Autoren zu Oberfl?chlichkeit
und „happy-go-lucky” Sicht verw?sserte, zeigt die vor allem unter europ?ischen Transaktionsanalytikern übliche Rückbesinnung
auf die ursprünglichen Inhalte den Umfang klinischer Relevanz der Konzepte für Verst?ndnis und Behandlung der verschiedensten
St?rungen. Dies anhand einer sog. frühen St?rung, dem Borderlinesyndrom, aufzuzeigen, ist Anliegen des vorliegenden Beitrags.
Das Strukturmodell der Ich-Zust?nde dient dabei als Beschreibungsansatz für die Borderlinestruktur, Bezugsrahmen und Skript als Muster für die Entstehung und Aufrechterhaltung der Pathologie, Transaktionen, Spiele, Symbiosen und Passivit?t als Merkmale stabiler Instabilit?t der Beziehungen und Abwertungen und Grandiosit?ten als Beispiele für Abwehrmuster. Es folgen praxisorientierte überlegungen und Hinweise zum therapeutischen Vertrag als Grundlage des Arbeitsbündnisses, zu Methoden der Ent?ngstigung im Therapieproze?, zu Begegnung und Beziehung als Substrat für korrigierende Erfahrungen, heilendes Klima und Entdramatisierung des realen Geschehens. Die Ausführungen
enden mit transaktionsanalytischen überlegungen zu übertragung, Gegenübertragung und dem Umgang mit Regressionsprozessen.
相似文献
102.
103.
Michaela Pfundmair Verena Graupmann Hongfei Du Dieter Frey Nilüfer Aydin 《International journal of psychology》2015,50(2):85-92
In the current research, we examined whether re‐inclusion (i.e. the change from a previous state of exclusion to a new state of inclusion) was perceived differently by people with individualistic and collectivistic cultural backgrounds. Individualists (German and Austrian participants) but not collectivists (Chinese participants) experienced re‐inclusion differently than continued inclusion: While collectivistic participants did not differentiate between both kinds of inclusion, individualistic participants showed reduced fulfilment of their psychological needs under re‐inclusion compared to continued inclusion. The results moreover revealed that only participants from individualistic cultures expressed more feelings of exclusion when re‐included than when continually included. These exclusionary feelings partially mediated the relationship between the different states of inclusion and basic need fulfilment. 相似文献
104.
FRANÇOIS SIROIS 《The Psychoanalytic quarterly》2015,84(1):159-167
105.
Terror management theory assumes that death arouses existential anxiety in humans which is suppressed in focal attention. Whereas most studies provide indirect evidence for negative affect under mortality salience by showing cultural worldview defenses and self‐esteem strivings, there is only little direct evidence for implicit negative affect under mortality salience. In the present study, we assume that this implicit affective reaction towards death depends on people's ability to self‐regulate negative affect as assessed by the personality dimension of action versus state orientation. Consistent with our expectations, action‐oriented participants judged artificial words to express less negative affect under mortality salience compared to control conditions whereas state‐oriented participants showed the reversed pattern. 相似文献
106.
The relationship between the Five‐Factor Model personality traits and peptic ulcer disease in a large population‐based adult sample 下载免费PDF全文
Anu Realo Andero Teras Liisi Kööts‐Ausmees Tõnu Esko Andres Metspalu Jüri Allik 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2015,56(6):693-699
The current study examined the relationship between the Five‐Factor Model personality traits and physician‐confirmed peptic ulcer disease (PUD) diagnosis in a large population‐based adult sample, controlling for the relevant behavioral and sociodemographic factors. Personality traits were assessed by participants themselves and by knowledgeable informants using the NEO Personality Inventory‐3 (NEO PI‐3). When controlling for age, sex, education, and cigarette smoking, only one of the five NEO PI‐3 domain scales – higher Neuroticism – and two facet scales – lower A1: Trust and higher C1: Competence – made a small, yet significant contribution (p < 0.01) to predicting PUD in logistic regression analyses. In the light of these relatively modest associations, our findings imply that it is certain behavior (such as smoking) and sociodemographic variables (such as age, gender, and education) rather than personality traits that are associated with the diagnosis of PUD at a particular point in time. Further prospective studies with a longitudinal design and multiple assessments would be needed to fully understand if the FFM personality traits serve as risk factors for the development of PUD. 相似文献
107.
108.
109.
110.
Müsseler J van der Heijden AH Mahmud SH Deubel H Ertsey S 《Perception & psychophysics》1999,61(8):1646-1661
We studied the ability to localize flashed stimuli, using a relative judgment task. When observers are asked to localize the peripheral position of a probe with respect to the midposition of a spatially extended comparison stimulus, they tend to judge the probe as being more toward the periphery than is the midposition of the comparison stimulus. We report seven experiments in which this novel phenomenon was explored. They reveal that the mislocalization occurs only when the probe and the comparison stimulus are presented in succession, independent of whether the probe or the comparison stimulus comes first (Experiment 1). The size of the mislocalization is dependent on the stimulus onset asynchrony (Experiment 2) and on the eccentricity of presentation (Experiment 3). In addition, the illusion also occurs in an absolute judgment task, which links mislocalization with the general tendency to judge peripherally presented stimuli as being more foveal than they actually are (Experiment 4). The last three experiments reveal that relative mislocalization is affected by the amount of spatial extension of the comparison stimulus (Experiment 5) and by its structure (Experiments 6 and 7). This pattern of results allows us to evaluate possible explanations of the illusion and to relate it to comparable tendencies observed in eye movement behavior. It is concluded that the system in charge of the guidance of saccadic eye movements is also the system that provides the metric in perceived visual space. 相似文献