全文获取类型
收费全文 | 179篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
186篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 5篇 |
2017年 | 2篇 |
2016年 | 2篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 20篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 5篇 |
2010年 | 3篇 |
2009年 | 2篇 |
2008年 | 4篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 3篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 4篇 |
2003年 | 7篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有186条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
L. M. Erickson S. T. Tiffany E. M. Martin T. B. Baker 《Behaviour research and therapy》1983,21(6):595-611
Rapid smoking (RS) and a less aversive rapid-puffing (RP) treatment were compared on ability to enhance the impact of a behavioral-counseling (BC) procedure on psychophysiological, attitudinal and behavioral response to cigarettes. Data reveal that both aversive smoking treatments resulted in less smoking once treatment began, reduced smoking in post-treatment taste tests, and superior follow-up performance at 6 months and 1 yr. At 1 yr, RS S s achieved the highest number of days abstinent of any group. Regression analyses showed that Ss' confidence ratings of remaining abstinent were highly predictive of follow-up status, and that these ratings were related to number of cigarettes smoked during treatment and amount of smoking during post-treatment taste tests. 相似文献
42.
43.
Richard C. Erickson Ph.D. Staff Psychologist 《Pastoral Psychology》1988,37(1):15-24
The increasing prevalence of chronic illness and chronic pain problems offers a challenge to pastors and churches. In this paper, notions of surplus suffering and disability are discussed, misconceived rules and expectations characterized, and strategies for helping chronic patients and their families described. 相似文献
44.
Erickson MA 《Memory & cognition》2008,36(4):749-761
One class of multiple-system models of category learning posits that within a single category-learning task people can learn to utilize different systems with different category representations to classify different stimuli. This is referred to as stimulus-dependent representation (SDR). The use of SDR implies that learners switch from subtask to subtask as trials demand. Thus, the use of SDR can be assessed via slowed response times, following a representation switch. Additionally, the use of SDR requires control of executive attention to keep inactive representations from interfering with the current response. Subjects were given a category learning task composed of one- and two-dimensional substructures. Control of executive attention was measured using a working memory capacity (WMC) task. Subjects most likely to be using SDR showed greater slowing of responses following a substructure switch and a greater correlation between learning performance and WMC. These results provide support for the principle of SDR in category learning and the reliance of SDR on executive attention. 相似文献
45.
Lee M. Joiner Edsel L. Erickson Wilbur B. Brookover 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1969,47(7):655-659
This is a study of changes in the educational plans of a longitudinal group of 255 males over grades 8, 9, 10, and 11. Subjected to testing and confirmed is the hypothesis that the student's perceived interaction with parents, regardless of socioeconomic status, is more directly associated with his educational plans than is socioeconomic status itself. The results also suggest that peers do not exceed parents in their influence upon adolescent plans when both sources of possible influence are measured by the same method. 相似文献
46.
47.
Thane M. Erickson Adam P. McGuire Gina M. Scarsella Tara A. Crouch Jamie A. Lewis Ashley P. Eisenlohr 《The journal of positive psychology》2018,13(6):643-654
Laboratory studies suggest that witnessing virtuous acts triggers moral elevation, an emotion characterized by feeling uplifted and increased prosocial motives. However, it remains unknown whether regular inductions can increase elevation and prosocial motives in daily life without being vulnerable to habituation or adaptation effects. Participants (total N = 181) were randomly assigned to elevating vs. neutral or amusing inductions (video or recall tasks) for seven days (Study 1), ten days (Study 2), or 12 days over one month (Study 3). Multilevel growth models showed that relative to comparison conditions, elevation inductions increased daily elevation, positive affect, and prosocial outcomes (compassionate goals and affiliation), and decreased self-protective motives (self-image goals). Adaptation effects occurred for immediate responses when exercises occurred daily, but not when spaced out over time, and not for outcomes measured later each day. Results suggest benefits of brief, regular elevation experiences and have implications for interventions targeting specific emotions. 相似文献
48.
Richard C. Erickson Ph.D. 《Journal of religion and health》1987,26(3):198-205
The resurgence of interest in spiritual and religious matters and in the depth psychology of C.G. Jung is examined relative to the work of spiritual direction and pastoral counseling. It is suggested that the risks of muddled thinking, narcissism, gnosticism, and credulity are increased when one attempts to combine spiritual disciplines with depth psychology. 相似文献
49.
Who is good at this game? Linking an activity to a social category undermines children's achievement
Children's achievement-related theories have a profound impact on their academic success. Children who adopt entity theories believe that their ability to perform a task is dictated by the amount of natural talent they possess for that task--a belief that has well-documented adverse consequences for their achievement (e.g., lowered persistence, impaired performance). It is thus important to understand what leads children to adopt entity theories. In the experiments reported here, we hypothesized that the mere act of linking success at an unfamiliar, challenging activity to a social group gives rise to entity beliefs that are so powerful as to interfere with children's ability to perform the activity. Two experiments showed that, as predicted, the performance of 4- to 7-year-olds (N = 192) was impaired by exposure to information that associated success in the task at hand with membership in a certain social group (e.g., "boys are good at this game"), regardless of whether the children themselves belonged to that group. 相似文献
50.
Previous studies have found that differences in brain volume among older adults predict performance in laboratory tasks of executive control, memory, and motor learning. In the present study we asked whether regional differences in brain volume as assessed by the application of a voxel-based morphometry technique on high resolution MRI would also be useful in predicting the acquisition of skill in complex tasks, such as strategy-based video games. Twenty older adults were trained for over 20 h to play Rise of Nations, a complex real-time strategy game. These adults showed substantial improvements over the training period in game performance. MRI scans obtained prior to training revealed that the volume of a number of brain regions, which have been previously associated with subsets of the trained skills, predicted a substantial amount of variance in learning on the complex game. Thus, regional differences in brain volume can predict learning in complex tasks that entail the use of a variety of perceptual, cognitive and motor processes. 相似文献