全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2621篇 |
免费 | 52篇 |
专业分类
2673篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 14篇 |
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 27篇 |
2020年 | 56篇 |
2019年 | 67篇 |
2018年 | 82篇 |
2017年 | 66篇 |
2016年 | 103篇 |
2015年 | 69篇 |
2014年 | 72篇 |
2013年 | 283篇 |
2012年 | 110篇 |
2011年 | 133篇 |
2010年 | 78篇 |
2009年 | 93篇 |
2008年 | 155篇 |
2007年 | 144篇 |
2006年 | 112篇 |
2005年 | 105篇 |
2004年 | 105篇 |
2003年 | 116篇 |
2002年 | 88篇 |
2001年 | 27篇 |
2000年 | 47篇 |
1999年 | 38篇 |
1998年 | 37篇 |
1997年 | 24篇 |
1996年 | 23篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 16篇 |
1993年 | 22篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 17篇 |
1990年 | 24篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 13篇 |
1972年 | 14篇 |
1971年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有2673条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
941.
942.
Research consistently links adolescents' and young adults' drinking with their peers' alcohol intake. In interpreting this correlation, 2 essential questions are often overlooked. First, which peers are more important, best friends or broader social networks? Second, do peers cause increased drinking, or do young people select friends whose drinking habits match their own? The present study combines social network analyses with family (twin and sibling) designs to answer these questions via data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Analysis of peer nomination data from 134 schools (n = 82,629) and 1,846 twin and sibling pairs shows that peer network substance use predicts changes in drinking from adolescence into young adult life even after controlling for genetic and shared environmental selection, as well as best friend substance use. This effect was particularly strong for high-intensity friendships. Although the peer-adolescent drinking correlation is partially explained by selection, the present finding offers powerful evidence that peers also cause increased drinking. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved). 相似文献
943.
Self-harm is one of the best predictors of death by suicide, but few studies directly compare adolescents with suicidal versus nonsuicidal self-harm. Seventy adolescents presenting with self-harm (71% young women, ages 12-18 years) who participated in a randomized controlled trial were divided into suicidal and nonsuicidal self-harm categories using the Columbia Classification Algorithm of Suicide Assessment. Adolescents with suicidal self-harm were more likely than those with nonsuicidal self-harm to be young women, 22/23 (96%) versus 34/47 (72%), odds ratio (OR) = 8.33, 95% confidence interval (CI) [1.03, 50.0]; had a later age of onset of self-harm, 15.4 years vs. 13.8 years, mean difference = 1.6, 95% CI [.8, 2.43]; and used self-poisoning more often, 18/23 (78%) versus 11/47 (23%), OR = 3.43, 95% CI [2.00, 5.89]. Only those with nonsuicidal self-harm had an improvement on Children's Global Assessment Scale score following a brief therapeutic intervention, mean difference = 8.20, 95% CI [.97, 15.42]. However, there was no interaction between treatment and suicidality. There are important differences between adolescents presenting with suicidal and nonsuicidal self-harm. Suicidal self-harm in adolescence may be associated with a less favorable response to therapeutic assessment. 相似文献
944.
Richard W. Puddy Michael C. Roberts Eric M. Vernberg Erin P. Hambrick 《Journal of child and family studies》2012,21(6):948-962
Coordination of mental health services in children with serious emotional disturbance (SED) has shown a preliminary relationship to positive outcomes in children. Yet, research in this area is sparse. Therefore, the relation between service coordination activities and adaptive functioning was examined for 51 children SED who were treated in the school-based Intensive Mental Health Program for elementary school students (IMHP; average treatment length was 12?months; mean age 9.37 at intake). Systematic review of detailed case records identified 16,669 episodes of service coordination that could be coded reliably for frequency, quality, and purpose (planning, linking, monitoring) of the service coordination component. Adaptive functioning was measured by Child and Adolescent Functional Assessment Scale (CAFAS) ratings and daily points earned on the behavior management system of the IMHP at intake, 6, 12?months, and discharge. Findings indicated that the frequency, quality, and purpose of the service coordination component varied from case to case and by time in treatment. Although service coordination activities overall decreased as treatment progressed, concentrated high quality service coordination (measured by frequency, quality, and component of service coordination) consistently predicted improved functioning on both CAFAS ratings and daily points earned. Results have both practice and policy implications for training interagency teams and clinicians on when and how to optimally coordinate services. 相似文献
945.
Past research has revealed that central vision is more important than peripheral vision in controlling the amplitude of target-directed aiming movements. However, the extent to which central vision contributes to movement planning versus online control is unclear. Since participants usually fixate the target very early in the limb trajectory, the limb enters the central visual field during the late stages of movement. Hence, there may be insufficient time for central vision to be processed online to correct errors during movement execution. Instead, information from central vision may be processed offline and utilised as a form of knowledge of results, enhancing the programming of subsequent trials. In the present research, variability in limb trajectories was analysed to determine the extent to which peripheral and central vision is used to detect and correct errors during movement execution. Participants performed manual aiming movements of 450 ms under four different visual conditions: full vision, peripheral vision, central vision, no vision. The results revealed that participants utilised visual information from both the central and peripheral visual fields to adjust limb trajectories during movement execution. However, visual information from the central visual field was used more effectively to correct errors online compared to visual information from the peripheral visual field. 相似文献
946.
Lowery BS Unzueta MM Knowles ED Goff PA 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2006,90(6):961-974
The present experiments suggest that the desire to benefit the in-group drives dominant-group members' policy preferences, independent of concern for out-groups' outcomes. In Experiment 1, the effect of a manipulation of affirmative action procedures on policy support was mediated by how Whites expected the policy to affect fellow Whites, but not by the expected effect on minorities. In Experiments 2 and 3, when focused on losses for the White in-group, Whites' racial identity was negatively related to support for affirmative action. However, when focused on gains for the Black out-group or when participants were told that Whites were not affected by the policy, racial identity did not predict attitudes toward the policy. In Experiments 2 and 3, perceived fairness mediated these effects. 相似文献
947.
Kenneth M. Tyler A. Wade Boykin Oronde Miller Eric Hurley 《Social Psychology of Education》2006,9(4):363-380
The present study examined the presence of specific cultural values within the preferred classroom and home activities of
African-American upper elementary students. Written scenarios were constructed and used to determine whether students preferred
their home and classroom activities carried out under specific cultural terms. Students also reported their perceptions of
teachers and parents’ cultural value-based preferences for classroom and home activities. With analysis of variance techniques,
it was shown that students and their parents have significantly stronger preferences for communal and vervistic activities
at home and at school than for individualistic and competitive activities. Perceived teacher classroom preferences, however,
were significantly higher for individualistic and competitive activities. Such findings underscore the presence of cultural
mismatch in the classroom experiences of African-Americans and illustrate a need to enhance school-based efforts to appreciate
and utilize cultural value variation. 相似文献
948.
This study assessed whether or not the difficulty of task switching stems from previous inhibition of the task set. A predictable
sequence of univalent stimuli (affording performance of one active task) and bivalent stimuli (affording performance of two
tasks) was used in two experiments. Experiment 1 used an alternating-runs paradigm (AABB) and Experiment 2 used a strictly
alternating sequence (ABAB). The critical variable was whether the incentive for task-set inhibition was strong (on bivalent
trials) or weak (on univalent trials). The question was whether it would be more difficult to switch to a task that previously
needed to be inhibited than to a task that did not need to be inhibited. This pattern was not observed in either experiment.
Thus, the data provide no evidence that task switching is difficult because of the need to overcome recent task-set inhibition. 相似文献
949.
Undermining trust and cooperation: The paradox of sanctioning systems in social dilemmas 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Laetitia B. Mulder Eric van Dijk Henk A.M. Wilke 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2006,42(2):147-162
Sanctioning systems in social dilemmas are often meant to increase trust in others and to increase cooperation. We argue, however, that sanctioning systems may also give people the idea that others act in their own self-interest and undermine the belief that others are internally motivated to cooperate. We developed the “Removing The Sanction” paradigm and a new trust manipulation, and showed in three experiments that when there is a sanction on defection, trust in others being internally motivated to cooperate is undermined: Participants who had experienced the presence of a sanctioning system trusted fellow group members less than participants who had not. In a similar vein, the sanction undermined cooperation when trust was initially high. The implications of these paradoxical findings are discussed. 相似文献
950.
Hagedoorn M Van Yperen NW Coyne JC van Jaarsveld CH Ranchor AV van Sonderen E Sanderman R 《Psychology and aging》2006,21(3):611-620
The association between marital status and distress was examined in a largely neglected group, namely older people (65 and older; N = 1,649). In this 2-wave study, married persons were less distressed than single persons, but perceived equity within the marriage and recency of bereavement qualified these findings. Married persons who felt inequitably treated were more distressed than persons who had always been single. Married persons reported less distress than recently (= 2 years ago) widowed persons, but only equitably treated married persons reported less distress than persons widowed for more than 2 years. Increased distress between the 2 waves occurred in recently widowed persons, and there was a decrease in distress in persons who were widowed shortly before the 1st wave. No gender effects were found. 相似文献