全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2343篇 |
免费 | 43篇 |
专业分类
2386篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 12篇 |
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 20篇 |
2021年 | 30篇 |
2020年 | 54篇 |
2019年 | 67篇 |
2018年 | 81篇 |
2017年 | 64篇 |
2016年 | 99篇 |
2015年 | 67篇 |
2014年 | 72篇 |
2013年 | 242篇 |
2012年 | 104篇 |
2011年 | 123篇 |
2010年 | 73篇 |
2009年 | 86篇 |
2008年 | 152篇 |
2007年 | 129篇 |
2006年 | 107篇 |
2005年 | 98篇 |
2004年 | 100篇 |
2003年 | 106篇 |
2002年 | 82篇 |
2001年 | 23篇 |
2000年 | 38篇 |
1999年 | 28篇 |
1998年 | 31篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 21篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 15篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 10篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有2386条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
Eric J. Mitnick 《Ethical Theory and Moral Practice》2004,7(2):163-177
Political theorists, increasingly, are realizing the virtues of contextuality to conceptual analysis. Just as theory may provide useful standards for the assessment of political practices, so may application of theoretical constructs within particular contexts provide a critical corrective to theory. This essay relates work undertaken within sociolegal studies applying a constitutive methodology to such efforts to contextualize political theorizing. The essay describes how the emphasis placed by constitutive theory on locality and meaning entails a contextual analysis. The essay then demonstrates how a constitutive and contextual approach can enhance our understanding of the moral issues surrounding differentiated citizenship policy. While the most obvious cost associated with differentiated citizenship policy is a loss in formal equality, a contextual assessment demonstrates the prospect of an even deeper, though ultimately contingent, moral loss in self-invention. 相似文献
112.
113.
114.
The goal of the study was to establish whether false allegations of sexual offences with an unknown perpetrator can be distinguished from accurate allegations. Case files of 27 true allegations of sexual offences with an unknown perpetrator were compared to those of 14 false allegations. The comparison was guided by a list of 43 criteria that are hypothesized to differentiate between true and false allegations of sexual assault. Analyses indicated the employed criteria differentiated true from false allegations to a certain extent. However, the discriminative strength of some criteria appeared to be stronger than that of others. Research is required to assess further the discriminative power of these criteria. 相似文献
115.
Eric Marcus 《Philosophical Studies》2005,122(1):27-50
It is generally accepted that the most serious threat to the possibility of mental causation is posed by the causal self-sufficiency of physical causal processes. I argue, however, that this feature of the world, which I articulate in principle I call Completeness, in fact poses no genuine threat to mental causation. Some find Completeness threatening to mental causation because they confuse it with a stronger principle, which I call Closure. Others do not simply conflate Completeness and Closure, but hold that Completeness, together with certain plausible assumptions, entails Closure. I refute the most fully worked-out version of such an argument. Finally, some find Completeness all by itself threatening to mental causation. I argue that one will only find Completeness threatening if one operates with a philosophically distorted conception of mental causation. I thereby defend what I call naïve realism about mental causation. 相似文献
116.
Eric L. G. Legge Christopher R. Madan Marcia L. Spetch Elliot A. Ludvig 《Animal cognition》2016,19(3):581-591
Encoding multiple cues can improve the accuracy and reliability of navigation and goal localization. Problems may arise, however, if one cue is displaced and provides information which conflicts with other cues. Here we investigated how pigeons cope with cue conflict by training them to locate a goal relative to two landmarks and then varying the amount of conflict between the landmarks. When the amount of conflict was small, pigeons tended to integrate both cues in their search patterns. When the amount of conflict was large, however, pigeons used information from both cues independently. This context-dependent strategy for resolving spatial cue conflict agrees with Bayes optimal calculations for using information from multiple sources. 相似文献
117.
118.
Comments on the article by E. Brandst?tter, G. Gigerenzer, and R. Hertwig. Resolution of debates in cognition usually comes from the introduction of constraints in the form of new data about either the process or representation. Decision research, in contrast, has relied predominantly on testing models by examining their fit to choices. The authors examine a recently proposed choice strategy, the priority heuristic, which provides a novel account of how people make risky choices. The authors identify a number of properties that the priority heuristic should have as a process model and illustrate how they may be tested. The results, along with prior research, suggest that although the priority heuristic captures some variability in the attention paid to outcomes, it fails to account for major characteristics of the data, particularly the frequent transitions between outcomes and their probabilities. The article concludes with a discussion of the properties that should be captured by process models of risky choice and the role of process data in theory development. 相似文献
119.
ABSTRACT There is no agreement regarding the nature or number of dimensions that make up the social effectiveness domain. We inductively explore the relationships between a set of social effectiveness measures with the intention of identifying an initial set of dimensions. An exploratory factor analysis of the Social Competence Inventory (SCI, Schneider, 2001 ) resulted in the identification of four factors: Social Potency, Social Appropriateness, Social Emotional Expression, and Social Reputation. A joint factor analysis between the SCI and a set of extant measures resulted in the identification of the same four factors. A fifth factor emerged when a set of scales from an emotional intelligence measure was included in the analysis, suggesting that emotional intelligence is not captured within the common factor space defined by measures of social effectiveness. This study represents a first step in the establishment of a set of common social effectiveness dimensions. 相似文献
120.
Salzer S Pincus AL Hoyer J Kreische R Leichsenring F Leibing E 《Journal of personality assessment》2008,90(3):292-299
In this study, we addressed the heterogeneity in interpersonal problems across patients with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). We assessed interpersonal problems by the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP-C; Horowitz, Alden, Wiggins, & Pincus, 2000) in a sample of 78 GAD patients. We used IIP-C profiles describing interpersonal characteristics of the total GAD sample as well as clustered GAD interpersonal subtypes. Although the overall sample was located in the friendly submissive quadrant of the circumplex model, this was true only for the Exploitable cluster, which includes more than 50% of the patients. Importantly, clusters of GAD patients with other locations reporting predominantly Cold, Nonassertive, or Intrusive interpersonal problems were also identified. The 4 clusters did not differ in terms of gender, comorbid disorders, or the severity of depression or anxiety. Thus, the assessment of interpersonal problems provides additional diagnostic information covering the heterogeneity of GAD patients. This information could be used for differential indication and individual case formulation in GAD. 相似文献