全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2294篇 |
免费 | 116篇 |
专业分类
2410篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 18篇 |
2021年 | 25篇 |
2020年 | 47篇 |
2019年 | 61篇 |
2018年 | 81篇 |
2017年 | 67篇 |
2016年 | 98篇 |
2015年 | 64篇 |
2014年 | 73篇 |
2013年 | 245篇 |
2012年 | 108篇 |
2011年 | 124篇 |
2010年 | 74篇 |
2009年 | 86篇 |
2008年 | 153篇 |
2007年 | 130篇 |
2006年 | 108篇 |
2005年 | 97篇 |
2004年 | 100篇 |
2003年 | 108篇 |
2002年 | 80篇 |
2001年 | 22篇 |
2000年 | 37篇 |
1999年 | 28篇 |
1998年 | 30篇 |
1997年 | 22篇 |
1996年 | 22篇 |
1995年 | 22篇 |
1994年 | 16篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 9篇 |
1969年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有2410条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
Recently, feminists like Jane Roland-Martin, Elizabeth Young-Bruehl, and others have advocated a conversational metaphor for thinking and rationality, and our image of the rational person. Elizabeth Young-Bruehl refers to thinking as a constant interconnecting of representations of experiences and an extension of how we hear ourselves and others. There are numerous disadvantages to thinking about thinking as a conversation.We think there are difficulties in accepting the current formulation of the conversational metaphor without question. First, there is danger that we will lose important dialectical connections like that between the self and society. Second, the conversational metaphor alone cannot fully express the way conversations are constructed. We will want to take up the notion of narrative as a metaphor for thinking advocated by Susan Bordo, Alasdair MacIntyre, Jerome Bruner, and others, including Mary Belenky and her colleagues.Eventually, we want to champion narrative and the dramatic narrative of culture as a metaphor for thinking that involves such expressions as sights, insights, silences, as well as sounds, moments of mood and poetic moments. The dramatic narrative provides the structural possibilities needed to criticize certain kinds of conversations, in order to talk about the relations of public and private, self and society and most importantly, about the drama of our lives within and without.The dramatic narrative for thinking helps dispel the dangerous dualisms of mind and body that not even conversation or narration alone can banish, and allows us to frame questions about education that do not require us to separate mind from body. The dramatic narrative metaphor for thinking lets us show who we are, act out what we think, and reconstruct rationality to reflect what many women, and some men, do. 相似文献
32.
At a crosswalk on a busy street, a confederate, sketching a drawing or not, stood close or far from a pedestrian for 10 sec before the street light indicated “Walk.” In the first study, pedestrians were given the opportunity to subsequently help this confederate or another accomplice by calling attention to a dropped pen. In the second study, crossing speeds were timed and pedestrians were asked to record their mood and reactions to “another pedestrian,” who happened to be the confederate. These studies tested and found evidence for both cognitive and arousal explanations of reactions to spatial invasions. Cognitive and attributional theories imply that characteristics of the invader are important mediators of reactions to the invasion. The close nonartist was judged to be more inappropriate, produced faster escape speeds, and was helped less than the close artist or the far confederates. Arousal explanations imply that any invasion produces a generalized reaction which would be unrelated to characteristics of the invader. The other accomplice was helped less when the subject had been invaded and this reduction in helping was unaffected by the artist manipulation. These findings suggest a general arousal response to any invasion with a subsequent reliance on cognitive interpretations. 相似文献
33.
34.
35.
36.
Previous studies have suggested an involvement of the midbrain ventral tegmental area in the biting attack upon a rat elicited by electrical stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus in cats. In order assess further the relationship between these two regions, 12 cats were implanted with attack-eliciting electrodes in both the lateral hypothalamus and the midbrain ventral tegmental area. Following a lesion of the midbrain attack site, attack previously elicited from hypothalamic electrodes ipsilateral to the lesion was eliminated or significantly reduced in frequency. The attack elicited from electrodes in the hypothalamus contralateral to the lesion was unaffected. Midbrain lesions made at sites from which attack was never elicited had no effect on hypothalamically elicited attack. The midbrain lesion in some cases eliminated only certain components of the total attack pattern; for example, the approach of a cat to the rat frequently remained present while the bite was absent. Additionally, it was found that the attack elicited from rostral hypothalamic electrodes was disrupted to a greater degree by a single midbrain lesion than the attack elicited from more caudal hypothalamic electrodes. These finding are discussed in terms of the neural system mediating this form of aggressive behavior in cats. 相似文献
37.
Kenneth R. Conner PsyD MPH Michael D. McCarthy PhD Alina Bajorska MA Eric D. Caine MD Xin M. Tu PhD Kerry L. Knox PhD 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2012,42(6):699-708
There are meager prospective data from nonclinical samples on the link between anxiety disorders and suicide or the extent to which the association varies over time. We examined these issues in a cohort of 309,861 U.S. Air Force service members, with 227 suicides over follow‐up. Mental disorder diagnoses including anxiety, mood, and substance‐use disorders (SUD) were based on treatment encounters. Risk for suicide associated with anxiety disorders were lower compared with mood disorders and similar to SUD. Moreover, the associations between mood and anxiety disorders with suicide were greatest within a year of treatment presentation. 相似文献
38.
Two experiments examined the effects of several types of repetition on state-dependent memory for conceptually categorized words. In both experiments, compatibility between pharmacological states at encoding and at retrieval facilitated the uncued recall of nonrepeated categories but had no appreciable effect on the recall of words within recalled categories. In both experiments, compatibility between encoding/retrieval states failed to facilitate the uncued recall of repeated categories li.e., categories whose names and/or exemplars were presented at least twice, in a constant or variable order, and with substantial spacing between successive presentations). And again, in both experiments, the level of uncued recall was higher for repeated than for nonrepeated categories, irrespective of the compatibility or incompatibility of encoding/retrieval states. These findings, together with other relevant observations reported in the literature, suggest that repetition in general, and repetition of category names in particular, influences recall in much the same ways as does explicit cuing with category names: It enhances the accessibility of higher order units, as reflected in uncued category recall, and it diminishes to the extent to which access to these units is state dependent. 相似文献
39.
Neuropsychology Review - Graph theory is a branch of mathematics that allows for the characterization of complex networks, and has rapidly grown in popularity in network neuroscience in recent... 相似文献
40.