首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   37860篇
  免费   819篇
  国内免费   7篇
  2020年   216篇
  2019年   234篇
  2018年   3767篇
  2017年   3068篇
  2016年   2577篇
  2015年   462篇
  2014年   408篇
  2013年   1417篇
  2012年   1139篇
  2011年   2923篇
  2010年   2847篇
  2009年   1823篇
  2008年   2226篇
  2007年   2700篇
  2006年   502篇
  2005年   676篇
  2004年   586篇
  2003年   493篇
  2002年   443篇
  2001年   731篇
  2000年   757篇
  1999年   501篇
  1998年   219篇
  1997年   203篇
  1996年   191篇
  1995年   155篇
  1994年   150篇
  1992年   371篇
  1991年   352篇
  1990年   379篇
  1989年   309篇
  1988年   296篇
  1987年   289篇
  1986年   268篇
  1985年   269篇
  1984年   259篇
  1983年   217篇
  1982年   167篇
  1979年   227篇
  1978年   180篇
  1977年   155篇
  1975年   200篇
  1974年   236篇
  1973年   235篇
  1972年   187篇
  1971年   189篇
  1969年   178篇
  1968年   212篇
  1967年   192篇
  1966年   159篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
71.
This study examined matching-to-sample procedures that might result in the emergence of conditional behavior never explicitly taught. Subjects were preschool children. Two pictures were displayed as comparisons on every trial, and samples were spoken words. In baseline training preceding each of three experiments, children learned to select pictures of a dog, a table, and a banana in response to their spoken English names. Thereafter, probe trials displayed novel comparisons with baseline comparisons: one novel comparison was displayed with the dog and another with the table. The three experiments differed primarily in the nature of the samples presented on probe trials. In Experiment 1, probe samples were novel words, "JAIJAI" and "BREEL." On the probes, each of seven subjects reliably selected the novel comparisons, apparently "excluding" the familiar ones. In Experiment 2, probe samples were from the subjects' baseline. On one probe, for example, the sample was "TABLE," and the subject had to choose either the dog or the novel picture. Exclusion was logically possible because the dog had always before been incorrect in the presence of "TABLE." Under these conditions, however, only two of nine children excluded reliably. In Experiment 3, probe samples were words that had never been samples on any matching-to-sample trial, but that had controlled the children's behavior in other settings. On one probe, for example, the sample was "PENCIL," and the subject had to choose either the dog or a novel picture. Subjects virtually always excluded the former and selected the latter. Unreliable exclusion in Experiment 2, therefore, apparently resulted because the probe samples had previously served also as samples on baseline trials. Spontaneous verbalizations recorded during probing provided further data consistent with this interpretation. The study helps to define variables controlling exclusion performances by showing that such performances are more likely to occur if the sample has no prior experimental history.  相似文献   
72.
We examined the functional role of verbalizations in the generalization of self-instructional training with preschoolers. Children learned to overtly self-instruct during classroom work periods prior to covert training. Data were collected on children's acquisition of verbal regulation during training and on overt use of self-instructions in the classroom generalization setting. Results of a multiple baseline design across subjects indicated that treatment effects were evident in the training setting but did not generalize to the classroom until children were emitting overt self-instructions in the classroom itself. The production of self-verbalizations in the generalization setting was related to changes in correct responding, on-task behavior, and efficiency in completing academic work.  相似文献   
73.
74.
75.
76.
77.
78.
T D Crespi 《Adolescence》1988,23(92):805-811
The effectiveness of a time-out intervention for adolescent psychiatric patients, adjudicated (delinquent) youth, and behaviorally disordered youngsters was explored in this study. The research was conducted in three psychiatric hospitals for children and adolescents, a facility for adjudicated youth, and in a day-treatment program. Utilizing a comparative outcome model, 813 occurrences of time-out with 274 youth were investigated in order to assess levels of effectiveness. The results indicated that time-out had a significant impact, and the use of a child-care specialist as time-out monitor improves overall effectiveness. Important implications for practicing professionals and clinical researchers are noted.  相似文献   
79.
80.
Memory and awareness in a patient with multiple personality disorder   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
We studied an individual with multiple personality disorder in whom each of several personalities claimed to have no direct awareness of the others and to be unable to consciously remember the experiences of other personalities. A broad selection of implicit and explicit memory tests was used to determine the extent to which one personality had access to knowledge acquired by another and the circumstances in which that knowledge would be expressed. The implicit assessment of memory was a necessary but not sufficient condition for demonstrating interpersonality access. The degree of compartmentalization of knowledge in this patient depended largely on whether the interpretation of presented information was likely to differ across personalities.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号