全文获取类型
收费全文 | 54167篇 |
免费 | 1673篇 |
国内免费 | 480篇 |
专业分类
56320篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 325篇 |
2020年 | 478篇 |
2019年 | 562篇 |
2018年 | 4015篇 |
2017年 | 3329篇 |
2016年 | 2919篇 |
2015年 | 743篇 |
2014年 | 739篇 |
2013年 | 2862篇 |
2012年 | 1762篇 |
2011年 | 3500篇 |
2010年 | 3148篇 |
2009年 | 2276篇 |
2008年 | 2704篇 |
2007年 | 3180篇 |
2006年 | 1056篇 |
2005年 | 1080篇 |
2004年 | 987篇 |
2003年 | 857篇 |
2002年 | 804篇 |
2001年 | 1123篇 |
2000年 | 1167篇 |
1999年 | 844篇 |
1998年 | 401篇 |
1997年 | 357篇 |
1996年 | 338篇 |
1995年 | 344篇 |
1992年 | 642篇 |
1991年 | 605篇 |
1990年 | 589篇 |
1989年 | 558篇 |
1988年 | 552篇 |
1987年 | 492篇 |
1986年 | 494篇 |
1985年 | 544篇 |
1984年 | 454篇 |
1983年 | 416篇 |
1982年 | 323篇 |
1979年 | 484篇 |
1978年 | 333篇 |
1976年 | 327篇 |
1975年 | 366篇 |
1974年 | 437篇 |
1973年 | 465篇 |
1972年 | 402篇 |
1971年 | 349篇 |
1969年 | 336篇 |
1968年 | 416篇 |
1967年 | 372篇 |
1966年 | 359篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Speech nonfluency in response to questions about the marital relationship was used to assess anxiety. Subjects were 31 husbands and 31 wives, all white, college educated, from middle- to lower-middle-class families, and ranging from 20 to 30 years of age. Three types of nonfluencies were coded: filled pauses, unfilled pauses, and repetitions. Speech-disturbance ratios were computed by dividing the sum of speech nonfluencies by the total words spoken. The results support the notion that some issues within marriage are more sensitive and/or problematic than others, and that, in an interview situation, gender interacts with question content in the production of nonfluencies. 相似文献
92.
A D Pick C F Palmer B L Hennessy M G Unze R K Jones R M Richardson 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1988,45(1):28-51
Children's perception of scale and contour in melodies was investigated in five studies. Experimental tasks included judging transposed renditions of melodies (Studies 1 and 3), discriminating between transposed renditions of a melody (Study 2), judging contour-preserving transformations of melodies (Study 4), and judging similarity to a familiar target melody of transformations preserving rhythm or rhythm and contour (Study 5). The first and second studies showed that young children detect key transposition changes even in familiar melodies and they perceive similarity over key transpositions even in unfamiliar melodies. Young children also are sensitive to melodic contour over transformations that preserve it (Study 5), yet they distinguish spontaneously between melodies with the same contour and different intervals (Study 4). The key distance effect reported in the literature did not occur in the tasks of this investigation (Studies 1 and 3), and it may be apparent only for melodies shorter or more impoverished than those used here. 相似文献
93.
Some observations on control by spoken words in children's conditional discrimination and matching by exclusion 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
This study examined matching-to-sample procedures that might result in the emergence of conditional behavior never explicitly taught. Subjects were preschool children. Two pictures were displayed as comparisons on every trial, and samples were spoken words. In baseline training preceding each of three experiments, children learned to select pictures of a dog, a table, and a banana in response to their spoken English names. Thereafter, probe trials displayed novel comparisons with baseline comparisons: one novel comparison was displayed with the dog and another with the table. The three experiments differed primarily in the nature of the samples presented on probe trials. In Experiment 1, probe samples were novel words, "JAIJAI" and "BREEL." On the probes, each of seven subjects reliably selected the novel comparisons, apparently "excluding" the familiar ones. In Experiment 2, probe samples were from the subjects' baseline. On one probe, for example, the sample was "TABLE," and the subject had to choose either the dog or the novel picture. Exclusion was logically possible because the dog had always before been incorrect in the presence of "TABLE." Under these conditions, however, only two of nine children excluded reliably. In Experiment 3, probe samples were words that had never been samples on any matching-to-sample trial, but that had controlled the children's behavior in other settings. On one probe, for example, the sample was "PENCIL," and the subject had to choose either the dog or a novel picture. Subjects virtually always excluded the former and selected the latter. Unreliable exclusion in Experiment 2, therefore, apparently resulted because the probe samples had previously served also as samples on baseline trials. Spontaneous verbalizations recorded during probing provided further data consistent with this interpretation. The study helps to define variables controlling exclusion performances by showing that such performances are more likely to occur if the sample has no prior experimental history. 相似文献
94.
Using a multi-trial, free-recall paradigm (Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test; Lezak, 1983), the acquisition and retention by learning disabled (LD) young adults with a Halstead Impairment Index less than or equal to 0.3 (low LD) or greater than or equal to 0.4 (high LD) was compared with that of nondisabled and head-injured (HI) young adults. Significant group differences, particularly during the last three acquisition trials, confirmed the effectiveness of this method in revealing subtle learning and retention deficits. Analyses of Trial 1 recall showed group differences in the prerecency but not the recency portion of the serial position curve, thus implicating long-term but not short-term memory differences between the groups. Analyses of pair frequency (Sternberg & Tulving, 1977) data showed that the three clinical groups had difficulty organizing the to-be-learned material for effective retrieval. Following an interpolated task, retention was poorer for the clinical groups than for the nondisabled and varied inversely with the degree of neuropsychological impairment. Finally, during retention, the nondisabled and low LD groups retained and used the list organization established during acquisition whereas the high LD and HI groups did not. 相似文献
95.
Herbert Sklar C.S.W. 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》1988,18(2):107-123
This paper emphasizes the impact of the setting, space, and general physical properties of the clinic upon the difficult patient's profound neediness and absence of inner regulatory structure. Moreover, the clinic's administrative style and requirements often related to fiscal restraints, demands for accountability for both how funds are spent and treatment outcome, all impinge upon the patient. Humanizing clinics so they have the capacity for psychotherapeutic holding and provision of safety for difficult patients are discussed in detail in this contribution. 相似文献
96.
The present study demonstrates the safety and effectiveness of an outpatient program designed to respond to adolescent-precipitated crises by mobilizing and restructuring the family's kinship system. Families of 75 adolescents at risk for hospitalization were followed up to 24 months after treatment with Systemic Crisis Intervention. Measures of offspring and family functioning, suicidal behavior, institutional use, and treatment costs are presented. Results clearly demonstrate the safety, effectiveness, and economic viability of Systemic Crisis Intervention. 相似文献
97.
An Expanded Attributional Style Questionnaire 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
98.
Return of fear: underlearning and overlearning 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
99.
100.
D C Beidel 《Journal of abnormal psychology》1988,97(1):80-82