全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1884篇 |
免费 | 26篇 |
专业分类
1910篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 23篇 |
2023年 | 29篇 |
2022年 | 20篇 |
2021年 | 36篇 |
2020年 | 54篇 |
2019年 | 63篇 |
2018年 | 70篇 |
2017年 | 82篇 |
2016年 | 66篇 |
2015年 | 41篇 |
2014年 | 51篇 |
2013年 | 237篇 |
2012年 | 56篇 |
2011年 | 83篇 |
2010年 | 49篇 |
2009年 | 53篇 |
2008年 | 63篇 |
2007年 | 60篇 |
2006年 | 48篇 |
2005年 | 51篇 |
2004年 | 37篇 |
2003年 | 30篇 |
2002年 | 45篇 |
2001年 | 27篇 |
2000年 | 40篇 |
1999年 | 28篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 12篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 19篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 17篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 19篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 17篇 |
1974年 | 24篇 |
1973年 | 22篇 |
排序方式: 共有1910条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
131.
Livesey EJ Thorwart A Harris JA 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2011,64(12):2316-2333
In human causal learning, positive patterning (PP) and negative patterning (NP) discriminations are often acquired at roughly the same rate, whereas PP is learned faster than NP in most experiments with nonhuman animals. One likely reason for this discrepancy is that most causal learning scenarios encourage participants to treat the presentation and omission of the relevant outcome as two events of comparable significance and likelihood. To investigate this, the current experiments compared PP and NP using a predictive learning paradigm based on a mock gambling task. In Experiment 1, one outcome (winning) was made more salient by being less frequent than the alternative outcome (losing). Under these circumstances, PP was learned faster than NP. In Experiment 2, subjects learned two PP and two NP discriminations, one involved win versus no change outcomes, the other involved lose versus no change outcomes. The subjects learned PP faster than NP, but only when discriminating win from no change. We argue that a difference in difficulty between PP and NP relies on a difference in the salience of the outcomes, consistent with the predictions of a relatively simple model of associative learning. 相似文献
132.
Harris AE 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》2011,59(4):701-736
This essay charts the origins, influences, and evolution of the relational tradition in contemporary psychoanalysis. Considering the theoretical and philosophical influences from nineteenth-century Americans like William James and C. S Pierce, and noting the seminal modern work of Steven Mitchell and Jay Greenberg in opening a critique of one-person focused drive theory, the essay follows developments over a quarter century. Hallmarks of the relational approach-social construction, two-person psychologies, multiple self-states, social regulation and construction of identities like gender and sexual orientation, and an evolving theory of clinical practice-are reviewed. New developments in clinical theory, in the study of identity categories, in the work on embodiment and enactment, and in developmental models are also reviewed. 相似文献
133.
134.
Laura Mickes Ryan S. Darby Vivian Hwe Daniel Bajic Jill A. Warker Christine R. Harris Nicholas J. S. Christenfeld 《Memory & cognition》2013,41(4):481-489
Online social networking is vastly popular and permits its members to post their thoughts as microblogs, an opportunity that people exploit, on Facebook alone, over 30 million times an hour. Such trivial ephemera, one might think, should vanish quickly from memory; conversely, they may comprise the sort of information that our memories are tuned to recognize, if that which we readily generate, we also readily store. In the first two experiments, participants’ memory for Facebook posts was found to be strikingly stronger than their memory for human faces or sentences from books—a magnitude comparable to the difference in memory strength between amnesics and healthy controls. The second experiment suggested that this difference is not due to Facebook posts spontaneously generating social elaboration, because memory for posts is enhanced as much by adding social elaboration as is memory for book sentences. Our final experiment, using headlines, sentences, and reader comments from articles, suggested that the remarkable memory for microblogs is also not due to their completeness or simply their topic, but may be a more general phenomenon of their being the largely spontaneous and natural emanations of the human mind. 相似文献
135.
Krystel Tossone Jeff Kretschmar Fredrick Butcher Leon Harris 《Journal of child and family studies》2016,25(4):1218-1228
Between 65 and 75 % of juvenile-justice involved (JJI) youth present with at least one behavioral health disorder. Many communities have developed diversion programs that provide behavioral health services to JJI youth, often in lieu of detention. A key component of successful diversion programming is accurate screening and assessment. The Ohio Scales, a validated instrument designed to track service effectiveness in clinical samples of youth, are now being used with juvenile justice populations. The purpose of this study is to validate the Ohio Scales in a JJI youth population (N = 2246). The population (ages 12–18) is derived from Ohio’s Behavioral Health Juvenile Justice Initiative, a diversion program for JJI youth with behavioral health issues. We conducted Confirmatory Factor Analyses (CFA) on all forms of the Ohio Scales (parent, youth and worker) to measure fit for one factor, four factor and four factor second order solutions. We also conducted an Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) on the Problem Severity factor in the youth form to determine the number of appropriate sub-factors. The EFA indicated that the Problem Severity factor should be broken into three hypothesized sub-factors: Externalizing, Internalizing and Delinquency. The CFA confirmed this solution. CFA results indicated the four factor second order solution fits superior to the other two solutions. Using the Ohio Scales Problem Severity measure as a three sub-factor measure may increase clinical applicability by allowing a clinician to specifically measure and target externalizing or internalizing issues during treatment. 相似文献
136.
Harris SM 《Journal of personality》2004,72(2):379-412
Associations among hardiness, health value, and health protective behaviors were examined as a function of ethnicity among 80 African American and 100 European American college students. The role of health value as moderator versus mediator in the hardiness-health behavior link was explored. Racial differences in correlations among hardiness, control, and commitment were found, with stronger relations for African American than European Americans. Moderate positive relations between hardiness, control, commitment, and health value for African American, as contrasted with weak relations between commitment and health value for European Americans, were also shown. A partial mediational effect for health value with personal distress and moderator effects for health value with personal distress and health habits were found for African American only. Race predicted hardiness variables, tobacco and alcohol use, personal distress, and health habits beyond what was accounted for by occupation and income. The ramifications of these data with regard to future studies on hardiness, health value, and health behaviors for African American are discussed. 相似文献
137.
Findings are presented of a study of families created through surrogacy arrangements. Forty-two surrogacy families were compared with 51 egg-donation families and 80 natural-conception families on standardized interview and questionnaire measures of the psychological well-being of the parents, the quality of parent-child relationships, and infant temperament. The differences that were identified between the surrogacy families and the other family types indicated greater psychological well-being and adaptation to parenthood by mothers and fathers of children born through surrogacy arrangements than by the natural-conception parents. 相似文献
138.
139.
Julie Aitken Harris J. Philippe Rushton Elizabeth Hampson Douglas N. Jackson 《Aggressive behavior》1996,22(5):321-331
Measures of salivary testosterone and the personality dimensions of aggression and pro-social behavior were obtained in 306 (155 male and 151 female) university students. Each participant provided two samples of saliva and completed ten self-report personality scales from multiple inventories. A factor analysis of the personality scales produced two factors, an aggression factor and a pro-social behavior factor. Men averaged five times the amount of salivary testosterone as women (99 pg/ml vs. 18.5 pg/ml) and rated themselves as more aggressive and less nurturant. Within each sex, testosterone was positively correlated with aggression and negatively correlated with pro-social personality. Structural equation modelling analyses suggested that a direct effect model best described the relationship between salivary testosterone and the latent personality dimensions of aggression and pro-social behavior. © 1996 Wiley-Liss, Inc. 相似文献
140.
Midline and off-midline tongue and right- and left-hand vibrotactile thresholds of stutterers and normal-speaking individuals 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Vibrotactile thresholds for 7 male stutterers and 7 normal-speaking men (age range for both groups = 19 to 32 yr.) were obtained from the right, midline, and left sides of the lingual dorsum, and the thenar eminences of both hands. The stutterers showed slightly higher (less sensitive) mean thresholds than the normal speakers for all three lingual test sites. This was not the case for the thresholds obtained from both hands. The preliminary data suggest that oral peripheral sensory integrity might be somewhat different for stutterers than for normal-speaking individuals. 相似文献