全文获取类型
收费全文 | 22331篇 |
免费 | 183篇 |
专业分类
22514篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 28篇 |
2022年 | 27篇 |
2021年 | 44篇 |
2020年 | 91篇 |
2019年 | 80篇 |
2018年 | 3551篇 |
2017年 | 2878篇 |
2016年 | 2331篇 |
2015年 | 257篇 |
2014年 | 129篇 |
2013年 | 302篇 |
2012年 | 658篇 |
2011年 | 2487篇 |
2010年 | 2565篇 |
2009年 | 1507篇 |
2008年 | 1777篇 |
2007年 | 2241篇 |
2006年 | 99篇 |
2005年 | 271篇 |
2004年 | 212篇 |
2003年 | 154篇 |
2002年 | 94篇 |
2001年 | 66篇 |
2000年 | 80篇 |
1999年 | 39篇 |
1998年 | 49篇 |
1997年 | 35篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 24篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 20篇 |
1991年 | 31篇 |
1990年 | 24篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 19篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 13篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 22篇 |
1976年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 10篇 |
1972年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
Rodger Beehler 《Studies in Philosophy and Education》1990,10(4):315-335
The essay examines the argument advanced by E.D. Hirsch, Jr., for instituting ‘cultural literacy’ as a fundamental priority of schools. A number of confusions and equivocations in Hirsch's reasoning are identified, and the propensity of his project to indoctrinate is exposed. Among the features of Hirsch's argument shown to be troubling are his shifting construal of ‘language’, his inconsistency about the requirements of cultural literacy, and his uncritical relation to traditional images of the American past and present. The upshot is to raise the question why Hirsch's project has elicited wide support and praise. 相似文献
32.
A time-sharing study used shadowing and interpretation/paraphrasing tasks to evaluate lateralization in professional interpreters individually matched to bilingual and monolingual controls. A two-step multivariate general linear model procedure was used to determine lateralized effects and extent of disruption produced by the tasks. Results revealed the monolingual group to be left lateralized for both tasks, but the two bilingual groups were lateralized in the LH only for shadowing. The monolingual group was significantly different from the bilingual groups in the pattern of hand asymmetry for interpretation/paraphrasing. The findings replicate outcomes of prior repeated measures analysis of variance procedures on percentage of change scores. However, new information is added by the more refined analysis. The findings are also discussed in terms of previous laterality studies using similar tasks and subject samples. 相似文献
33.
34.
35.
Jörg R. J. Schirra 《Kognitionswissenschaft》1997,6(4):177-195
How can we explain that an assertion on something perceived can be understood in the same manner by somebody who cannot perceive that scene? This problem bases the interest in computational linguistics in how listener modeling could possibly be harmonized with reference semantics. Mental images substituting real perception appear as a way out. The architecture of the listener model has to be adapted to the creation and use of such pictorial data structures. Furthermore, the relation between the latter and a verbal (i. e., propositional) representation must be understood. The resulting architecture of a listener model with reference semantics can be employed to solve communicational problems from three general classes in a better way, as is demonstrated by an example implementation. 相似文献
36.
Kenneth J. Zucker Richard Green Christina Garofano Susan J. Bradley Katherine Williams Howard M. Rebach Claire B. Lowry Sullivan 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1994,22(1):1-13
A mother's (prenatal) wish for a girl has, at times, been implicated as an etiological factor for boyhood femininity or its DSM-III-R diagnostic equivalent, the Gender Identity Disorder of Childhood. In one sample of 52 feminine and 52 nonclinical control boys from Los Angeles, the proportion of mothers who recalled a wish for a girl during the pregnancy did not differ significantly between groups (26.9% vs. 19.2%, respectively); however, in both groups, the maternal wish for a girl was significantly more common in sibships in which all of the proband's older siblings were male. In a second sample of 103 feminine boys from Toronto, the maternal wish for a girl was significantly higher than that found in the Los Angeles sample (43.7% vs. 26.9%), but a control group was not available; as in the Los Angeles sample, the maternal wish for a girl was significantly more common in sibships in which all of the proband's older siblings were male. Implications of these findings for further study of maternal influences on boyhood femininity are discussed. 相似文献
37.
38.
Subjects chose between pairs of hypothetical amounts of money available after different delays. When smaller, more immediate amounts were selected over larger, more delayed amounts, the addition of a constant delay to both outcomes resulted in reversals of preference, contrary to the standard discounted utility model of economics. The delays at which preference reversed were determined for three pairs of amounts ($20 vs. $50, $100 vs. $250, and $500 vs. $1,250). The relation between the delay to the larger amount and the delay to the smaller amount at preference reversal was well fit by both linear and quadratic functions. Intercepts increased with amount, strongly suggesting that rate of discounting decreases with amount. The presence of significant negative curvature in the data from the majority of individual subjects poses problems for exponential and hyperbolic models of temporal discounting in self-control, both of which predict a linear relation between the delays to the larger and smaller amounts. 相似文献
39.
Robert G. Green Ph.D. Dean Braley M.S. Anne Kisor Ph.D. 《Journal of child and family studies》1996,5(3):267-283
We tested the notion that better foster care adjustment would be observed when the temperaments of mothers and fathers were matched with those of adolescent foster children. We hypothesized that families in which foster parents and foster children had high (easy) scores on subscales of the Revised Dimensions of Temperament Scale (DOTS-R) would also report higher family functioning and higher foster care adjustment than parents and children from families in which one or both family members had low (difficult) scores on these scales. The hypotheses were supported when the mothers and fathers family assessments were used but rejected when the observations of the case managers and adolescents were tested. Methodological and substantive explanations for the findings are discussed and recommendation for foster care practice and continuing research are provided. 相似文献
40.
William D. Murphy Mary R. Haynes Emily M. Coleman Barry Flanagan 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1985,7(1):37-47
The present study presents data relevant to the sexual responding to aggressive cues of a large sample of nonrapist males recruited from the community. Two hundred three subjects received physiological assessment of sexual arousal to heterosexual and rape stimuli. Results indicated that under instructions not to interfere with sexual responding, approximately 80% of the nonrapists would be correctly classified, which was significantly different from chance. However, under instructions to suppress arousal, classification was no better than chance. The rape index was not related to age, socioeconomic status, sexual experience, or amount of arousal shown in the laboratory. However, there was a small but significant relationship to IQ. Overall, the data suggest that for instructions not to interfere with responding, the error rate seen in this larger-scale sample was equivalent to that in previous studies using smaller normative samples and that classification in general is not biased by the subject characteristics measured in this study.Portions of this project were supported by USPHS Grant MH-34030-02 from the Center for the Studies of Crime and Delinquency and by Tennessee Department of Mental Health and Mental Retardation Research Contract ID-0783 to the Department of Psychiatry. 相似文献