首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   156篇
  免费   9篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   3篇
  2020年   2篇
  2019年   6篇
  2018年   10篇
  2017年   5篇
  2016年   10篇
  2014年   7篇
  2013年   21篇
  2012年   8篇
  2011年   10篇
  2010年   10篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   7篇
  2007年   4篇
  2006年   12篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   4篇
  2003年   2篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   1篇
  1997年   2篇
  1995年   1篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   3篇
  1970年   2篇
  1968年   1篇
排序方式: 共有165条查询结果,搜索用时 218 毫秒
141.
Objective: Goal-concordant care is an important feature of high quality medical treatment. Patients’ care goals may focus on curative and/or palliative outcomes. Patients rarely communicate their care goals, and providers’ predictions of patient goals are often inaccurate, corresponding most closely to their own treatment goals. This projection of own goals onto patients introduces the potential for bias, leading to goal-discordant care.

Design and Main Outcomes: We examined goal discordance using data from a U.S. sample of healthcare providers (N?=?492) recruited online in 2017 using GfK Knowledge Panel. Providers reported their perceptions of their patients’ care goals (curative relative to palliative), their own care goals if they were to become ill, and their willingness to deliver palliative care.

Results: For 28% of providers, their own care goals differed from their patients’. Providers were more likely to prioritise palliative care (relative to curative) in their own goals than in their predictions about patients’ goals. Providers were more willing to deliver palliative care when their own goals prioritised more palliative relative to curative care, but their perceptions of patient goals were unassociated with willingness to provide it.

Conclusions: Efforts to improve goal communication and reduce projection biases among providers may facilitate goal-concordant care.  相似文献   

142.
143.
Three experiments are presented that compare the cost found when switching from one task to another in two different conditions. In one of them, the tasks switch in predictable sequences. In the other condition, the tasks alternate at random. A smaller time cost is found in the random-switch condition when enough preparation time is allowed. Such an effect is due to the random-switch cost continuing to decrease with preparation time after the predictable-switch cost has reached an asymptote. Although the relationship between number of repetitions of one task and time cost is different in the random- and the predictable-switch conditions, only the latter shows the presence of an "exogenous" component of cost. The implications of this finding are discussed in relationship with the usual distinction between an endogenous component of switch cost that is affected by preparation time and another exogenous, residual component (e.g., Rogers & Monsell, 1995). It is proposed that a different kind of task-set preparation is at work when tasks alternate at random.  相似文献   
144.
145.
146.
147.
在1974年的一次访谈中,拉康针对当时人们越来越多地谈到精神分析学的危机问题,明确回答说,弗洛伊德的学说并没有过时,精神分析学也远远没有走到尽头。不过,当时的精神分析学界确实也出现了一些偏离,后辈们并不总是忠实于他们的鼻祖弗洛伊德,精神分析学面临着被教条化、心理学化和平庸化的危险处境。对此,他再次呼吁要回到弗洛伊德去。弗洛伊德认为,精神分析学主要是一门实践学科,是言语的实践,并非行为的治疗。患者的言语,是精神分析的唯一材料。  相似文献   
148.
The aim of this paper is to present the principles of intersubjectivity as a second-person relational account of mind, set against individualist first- and third-person accounts of the sharing of mental representations. I will set out a summary of these positions and defend the claim that understanding proto-conversations as “expressive communications” allows one to understand them as genuine communications, as in, mutually manifest communications that entail intersubjectivity. To support this interpretation, I will propose a novel explanation of expression, understood as constitutive of the mental state.  相似文献   
149.
150.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号